首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
   检索      

个体认知方式与材料复杂性对视空间工作记忆的影响
引用本文:李寿欣,周颖萍.个体认知方式与材料复杂性对视空间工作记忆的影响[J].心理学报,2006,38(4):523-531.
作者姓名:李寿欣  周颖萍
作者单位:山东师范大学教科院心理系,济南 250014
基金项目:山东省泰山学者建设工程专项经费资助项目。
摘    要:采用计算机呈现的Corsi积木点击任务,探讨了不同认知方式个体对材料的路径、结构和数量复杂性不同的视空间工作记忆的广度。研究结果表明:(1)场独立性认知方式是影响视空间工作记忆广度的一个重要的个别差异变量,在路径复杂或呈现的材料结构随机的条件下,场独立者的视空间工作记忆广度明显高于场依存者;(2)材料呈现的路径、数量是影响视空间记忆广度的重要因素,在路径简单或数量少的情况下被试的视空间记忆广度要明显得高

关 键 词:认知方式  视空间工作记忆  结构复杂性  数量复杂性  路径复杂性  
收稿时间:2005-06-08
修稿时间:2005年6月8日

The Influence of Individual Cognitive Style and Material Complexity on Visuo-Spatial Working Memory
Li Shouxin,Zhou Yingping.The Influence of Individual Cognitive Style and Material Complexity on Visuo-Spatial Working Memory[J].Acta Psychologica Sinica,2006,38(4):523-531.
Authors:Li Shouxin  Zhou Yingping
Institution:Department of Psychology, Educational Science College of Shandong Normal University, Jinan 250014, China
Abstract:Introduction: Researchers have recently paid attention to individual differences in visuo-spatial working memory. An important issue is the influence of several individual difference variables, such as gender, age, study difficulty, spatial ability, on visuo-spatial working memory. In the present study, we investigated whether there were differences on visuo-spatial working memory among individuals of different cognitive styles when the memory materials were different in difficulty. Method:Experiment 1: (1) Participants: Fifty-six graduates participated in the GEFT. Among them, 16 (top 30%) with highest scores were defined as field-independent, and 16 (bottom 30%) as field-dependent. The final sample consisted of 32 participants (14 males, 18 females; mean age = 20.2 years). (2) Experimental design: This was a mixed design of two factors which were cognitive style (between-group) and path complexity (within-group). The cognitive style included field-independence and field-dependence, whereas the path complexity included simple path and complex path. The dependent variable was the span of visuo-spatial working memory which could be obtained when the total sum of the three biggest spatial orders was divided by three. (3) Materials and equipments: There were 25 cubes used in Corsi block-tapping task, which were presented on the computer screen with a blue background (23×25cm) in the shape of a 5×5 matrix. Experiment 2: (1) Participants: Fifty-five graduates participated initially in the GEFT. The method used was the same as that of Experiment 1. The final sample included 16 field-independent and 16 field-dependent students (15 males, 17 females; mean age = 20.8 years). (2) Experimental design: This was a mixed design of three factors which were cognitive style (between-group ), structural complexity (within-group) and quantitative complexity (within-group). The cognitive style included field-independent and field-dependent, the structural complexity included the matrix shape and random shape, and quantitative complexity included the small quantity and the large quantity. The dependent variable was the same as that in experiment 1. (3) Materials and equipments: In Corsi block-tapping task, the cubes were the same as those in experiment 1. Quantitative complexity included both the small quantity (9 cubes) and the large quantity (16 cubes). The structural complexity included matrix shape and random shape. We used SPSS11.5 to analyze the data of the two experiments. Results: Table 1 The span of memory of different cognitive style in different path complexity tasks (M±SD) cognitive style simple path complex path field independence 6.146±1.068 5.020±1.013 field dependence 5.751±1.118 3.895±0.665 An MANOVA was administered on the span of visuo-spatial working memory. The results indicated that the main effect of path complexity was significant , F(1,30)=73.612, p<0.001;under the simpler path, the span of memory was significantly higher than that of the complex path; the main effect of cognitive style was significant , F(1,30)=6.392, p<0.05; the field-independent subjects’ span of visuo-spatial working memory was significantly higher than that of the field-dependent subjects; the interaction between cognitive style and path complexity was significant , F(1,30)=4.410, p<0.05. Table2. The span of memory of different cognitive style under different experiment conditions(M±SD) experiment small quantity large quantityconditions matrix random matrix random field independence 6.125±1.039 6.271±1.063 5.771±0.757 5.917±1.145 field dependence 6.083±1.000 5.167±0.699 5.396±0.743 4.875±0.383 After an MANOVA test on the span of visuo-spatial working memory, we obtained the following results. The main effect of quantitative complexity was significant ,F(1,30)=8.129, p<0.01; under the small quantity condition, the span of visuo-spatial working memory was significantly higher than that of the large quantity; the main effect of cognitive style was significant, F(1,30)=9.816, p<0.01; the field-independent subjects’ span of visuo-spatial working memory was significantly higher than that of the field-dependent subjects; the interaction between cognitive style and structural complexity was significant , F(1,30)=9.317,
Keywords:cognitive style  visuo-spatial working memory  structural complexity  quantitative complexity  path complexity  
本文献已被 CNKI 维普 万方数据 等数据库收录!
点击此处可从《心理学报》浏览原始摘要信息
点击此处可从《心理学报》下载免费的PDF全文
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号