首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   4238篇
  免费   253篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2024年   7篇
  2023年   39篇
  2022年   39篇
  2021年   74篇
  2020年   101篇
  2019年   134篇
  2018年   197篇
  2017年   214篇
  2016年   196篇
  2015年   125篇
  2014年   154篇
  2013年   473篇
  2012年   279篇
  2011年   271篇
  2010年   145篇
  2009年   170篇
  2008年   251篇
  2007年   238篇
  2006年   179篇
  2005年   162篇
  2004年   162篇
  2003年   168篇
  2002年   142篇
  2001年   55篇
  2000年   49篇
  1999年   46篇
  1998年   52篇
  1997年   42篇
  1996年   37篇
  1995年   38篇
  1994年   25篇
  1993年   29篇
  1992年   17篇
  1991年   15篇
  1990年   15篇
  1989年   12篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   16篇
  1986年   9篇
  1985年   18篇
  1984年   8篇
  1983年   9篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   6篇
  1978年   6篇
  1968年   4篇
  1939年   3篇
  1937年   4篇
排序方式: 共有4493条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
The current study was designed to gain a better understanding of the nature of the relationship between substance use and sexual risk taking within a community sample of women (N = 1,004). Using confirmatory factor analysis, the authors examined the factor structure of sexual risk behaviors and substance use to determine whether they are best conceptualized as domains underlying a single, higher order, risk-taking propensity. A 2 higher order factor model (sexual risk behavior and substance use) provided the best fit to the data, suggesting that these 2 general risk domains are correlated but independent factors. Sensation seeking had large general direct effects on the 2 risk domains and large indirect effects on the 4 first-order factors and the individual indicators. Negative affect had smaller, yet still significant, effects. Impulsivity and anxiety were unrelated to sexual health risk domains.  相似文献   
7.
8.
Developing readers often make anagrammatical errors (e.g. misreading pirates as parties), suggesting they use letter position flexibly during word recognition. However, while it is widely assumed that the occurrence of these errors decreases with increases in reading skill, empirical evidence to support this distinction is lacking. Accordingly, we compared the performance of developing child readers (aged 8–10 years) against the end‐state performance of skilled adult readers in a timed naming task, employing anagrams used previously in this area of research. Moreover, to explore the use of letter position by developing readers and skilled adult readers more fully, we used anagrams which, to form another word, required letter transpositions over only interior letter positions, or both interior and exterior letter positions. The patterns of effects across these two anagram types for the two groups of readers were very similar. In particular, both groups showed similarly slowed response times (and developing readers increased errors) for anagrams requiring only interior letter transpositions but not for anagrams that required exterior letter transpositions. This similarity in the naming performance of developing readers and skilled adult readers suggests that the end‐state skilled use of letter position is established earlier during reading development than is widely assumed.  相似文献   
9.
ABSTRACT The central claim is that Hornsby's argument that semantic knowledge is practical knowledge is based upon a false premise. I argue, contra Hornsby, that speakers do not voice their thoughts directly. Rather, our actions of voicing our thoughts are justified by decisions we make (albeit rapidly) about what words to use. Along the way, I raise doubts about other aspects of the thesis that semantic knowledge is practical knowledge.  相似文献   
10.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号