首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3352篇
  免费   232篇
  2024年   7篇
  2023年   41篇
  2022年   53篇
  2021年   83篇
  2020年   129篇
  2019年   145篇
  2018年   202篇
  2017年   196篇
  2016年   178篇
  2015年   136篇
  2014年   132篇
  2013年   374篇
  2012年   214篇
  2011年   209篇
  2010年   109篇
  2009年   134篇
  2008年   162篇
  2007年   158篇
  2006年   121篇
  2005年   96篇
  2004年   100篇
  2003年   61篇
  2002年   67篇
  2001年   38篇
  2000年   29篇
  1999年   33篇
  1998年   27篇
  1997年   21篇
  1996年   18篇
  1995年   11篇
  1994年   16篇
  1993年   16篇
  1992年   22篇
  1991年   18篇
  1990年   14篇
  1989年   23篇
  1988年   14篇
  1987年   12篇
  1986年   16篇
  1985年   18篇
  1984年   14篇
  1983年   9篇
  1982年   13篇
  1981年   9篇
  1980年   7篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   10篇
  1972年   6篇
  1968年   9篇
  1967年   9篇
排序方式: 共有3584条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
4.
It is widely assumed that reinforcers are biologically relevant stimuli, or stimuli that have been associated with biologically relevant stimuli. However, brief, arbitrary stimuli have also been reported to have reinforcement-like effects, despite being unrelated to biologically relevant stimuli like food. The present study explored the potential reinforcement-like effects of brief stimuli across 5 experiments. In Experiments 1 through 4, pigeon subjects responded for food reinforcement and brief stimulus presentations in a 2-component multiple schedule. Neither baseline response rates nor resistance to change during disruption tests were systematically greater in a component with versus without brief stimulus presentations. Increasing the rate and duration of brief stimulus presentations in Experiment 4 did not reveal reinforcement-like effects when compared directly with food. In Experiment 5, pigeons chose between independent terminal links in a concurrent-chains procedure. Across conditions, varying the location, duration, and rate of brief stimulus presentations in the terminal links had no systematic effects on preference. In contrast, varying rates of food reinforcers resulted in large and reliable shifts in preference. Therefore, the present study found no systematic evidence that brief stimuli unrelated to food reliably increase response rates, resistance to change, or preference. These data demonstrate the value of systematic replication, and a behavioral momentum approach to assessing potential reinforcement-like effects.  相似文献   
5.
6.
7.
8.
How do speakers design what they say in order to communicate effectively with groups of addressees who vary in their background knowledge of the topic at hand? Prior findings indicate that when a speaker addresses a pair of listeners with discrepant knowledge, that speakers Aim Low, designing their utterances for the least knowledgeable of the two addressees. Here, we test the hypothesis that speakers will depart from an Aim Low approach in order to efficiently communicate with larger groups of interacting partners. Further, we ask whether the cognitive demands of tracking multiple conversational partners' perspectives places limitations on successful audience design. We find that speakers can successfully track information about what up to four of their partners do and do not know in conversation. When addressing groups of 3–4 addressees at once, speakers design language based on the combined knowledge of the group. These findings point to an audience design process that simultaneously represents the perspectives of multiple other individuals and combines these representations in order to design utterances that strike a balance between the different needs of the individuals within the group.  相似文献   
9.
10.
The Family System Functioning (FSF) scale is a new instrument for measuring dimensions of the intrafamilial environment thought to be important in recovery from major psychiatric illness. Modest statistical correlations were obtained when FSF ratings of laboratory-based family interactions were compared with researcher-guided therapist ratings of FSF based upon the family's behavior in family therapy sessions during the subsequent month. The data from these two settings provide support for the validity of some of the scales. Because of the modest size of the correlations, however, behavior in the laboratory setting may not always be an accurate indicator of how the family will behave in the early weeks of family therapy.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号