首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   35篇
  免费   2篇
  2020年   1篇
  2017年   1篇
  2013年   4篇
  2012年   3篇
  2011年   4篇
  2010年   4篇
  2008年   2篇
  2007年   1篇
  2005年   4篇
  2004年   2篇
  2001年   2篇
  2000年   1篇
  1995年   1篇
  1991年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   2篇
排序方式: 共有37条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
1.
Supported by a review of relevant literature and a case study, the author argues that family therapy intervention enhances the likelihood of a female alcoholic's positive prognosis. With a basis in systems theory, family therapy views the family as a system and, hence, perceives the alcoholism as a symptom of that family. A profile of the female alcoholic, how her drinking affects her family, and the role that family therapy plays in the recovery process are examined.  相似文献   
2.
Single neuron recording studies have demonstrated the existence of hippocampal spatial view neurons which encode information about the spatial location at which a primate is looking in the environment. These neurons are able to maintain their firing even in the absence of visual input. The standard neuronal network approach to model networks with memory that represent continuous spaces is that of continuous attractor networks. It has recently been shown how idiothetic (self-motion) inputs could update the activity packet of neuronal firing for a one-dimensional case (head direction cells), and for a two-dimensional case (place cells which represent the place where a rat is located). In this paper, we describe three models of primate hippocampal spatial view cells, which not only maintain their spatial firing in the absence of visual input, but can also be updated in the dark by idiothetic input. The three models presented in this paper represent different ways in which a continuous attractor network could integrate a number of different kinds of velocity signal (e.g., head rotation and eye movement) simultaneously. The first two models use velocity information from head angular velocity and from eye velocity cells, and make use of a continuous attractor network to integrate this information. A fundamental feature of the first two models is their use of a 'memory trace' learning rule which incorporates a form of temporal average of recent cell activity. Rules of this type are able to build associations between different patterns of neural activities that tend to occur in temporal proximity, and are incorporated in the model to enable the recent change in the continuous attractor to be associated with the contemporaneous idiothetic input. The third model uses positional information from head direction cells and eye position cells to update the representation of where the agent is looking in the dark. In this case the integration of idiothetic velocity signals is performed in the earlier layer of head direction cells.  相似文献   
3.
Neurons were found in the rhesus macaque anterior orbitofrontal cortex that respond to novel but not to familiar visual stimuli. Some of these neurons responded to all novel stimuli, and others to only a subset (e.g., to novel faces). The neurons have no responses to familiar reward- or punishment-associated visual stimuli, nor to taste, olfactory or somatosensory inputs. The responses of the neurons typically habituated with repeated presentations of a novel stimulus, and five presentations each 1s was the median number for the response to reach half-maximal. The neurons did not respond to stimuli which had been novel and shown a few times on the previous day, indicating that the neurons were involved in long-term memory. The median latency of the neuronal responses was 120 ms. The median spontaneous firing rate was 1.3 spikes/s, and the median response to novel visual stimuli was 6.0 spikes/s. These findings indicate that the long-term memory for visual stimuli is information that is represented in a region of the primate anterior orbitofrontal cortex.  相似文献   
4.
Alistair Rolls 《Sophia》2011,50(4):527-541
This paper demonstrates that L'étranger, Camus's famous novel about an outsider, had by as early as 1946 become just as much of an 'insider' in terms of its affiliation to the Parisian literary tradition. More than an insider simply by virtue of its contemporary place in the French canon, then, the novel is also intertextually bound to a tradition of oxymoronic poetics dating back to Charles Baudelaire's Paris Spleen (Les Petits poèmes en prose). I shall examine the way in which L'étranger performs its prose poetics, thereby establishing it as exemplary of a Parisian model of modernity. Additionally, the famous scene on the beach will be considered as a liminal space and as a literary translation of Paris into the desert, which, once a joke for Paris's relationship to provincial France, became after the Second World War a new allegory for the capital's self-alterity.  相似文献   
5.
Skeptical worries about moral responsibility seem to be widely appreciated and deeply felt. To address these worries—if nothing else to show that they are mistaken—theories of moral responsibility need to relate to whatever concept of responsibility underlies the worries. Unfortunately, the nature of that concept has proved hard to pin down. Not only do philosophers have conflicting intuitions; numerous recent empirical studies have suggested that both prosaic responsibility judgments and incompatibilist intuitions among the folk are influenced by a number of surprising factors, sometimes prompting apparently contradictory judgments. In this paper, we show how an independently motivated hypothesis about responsibility judgments provides a unified explanation of the more important results from these studies. According to this ‘Explanation Hypothesis’, to take an agent to be morally responsible for an event is to take a relevant motivational structure of the agent to be part of a significant explanation of the event. We argue that because of how explanatory interests and perspectives affect what we take as significant explanations, this analysis accounts for the puzzling variety of empirical results. If this is correct, the Explanation Hypothesis also provides a new way of understanding debates about moral responsibility.  相似文献   
6.
Rolls ET 《Perception》2008,37(3):333-354
Top-down perceptual influences can bias (or pre-empt) perception. In natural scenes, the receptive fields of neurons in the inferior temporal visual cortex (IT) shrink to become close to the size of objects. This facilitates the read-out of information from the ventral visual system, because the information is primarily about the object at the fovea. Top-down attentional influences are much less evident in natural scenes than when objects are shown against blank backgrounds, though are still present. It is suggested that the reduced receptive-field size in natural scenes, and the effects of top-down attention contribute to change blindness. The receptive fields of IT neurons in complex scenes, though including the fovea, are frequently asymmetric around the fovea, and it is proposed that this is the solution the IT uses to represent multiple objects and their relative spatial positions in a scene. Networks that implement probabilistic decision-making are described, and it is suggested that, when in perceptual systems they take decisions (or 'test hypotheses'), they influence lower-level networks to bias visual perception. Finally, it is shown that similar processes extend to systems involved in the processing of emotion-provoking sensory stimuli, in that word-level cognitive states provide top-down biasing that reaches as far down as the orbitofrontal cortex, where, at the first stage of affective representations, olfactory, taste, flavour, and touch processing is biased (or pre-empted) in humans.  相似文献   
7.
Each year, numerous children testify in the United States in either criminal, civil, or juvenile court cases. Typically, children who testify are alleged victims of sexual or physical abuse or neglect, witnesses to violent crime, or subjects in custodial hearings in civil court. As more maltreatment cases are prosecuted and child custody is contested, an increasing number of children are being called as witnesses in court. Many of these children have already been traumatized by the experiences that led to their need to testify, and participation in the court process can have additional negative effects. This article describes the development and recent formative evaluation of a court preparation program that provides psychoeducation and utilizes components of evidence-based trauma treatment approaches, such as relaxation and in vivo exposure, to support child witnesses and reduce the risk of retraumatization. Children and adolescents participating in the program attend group sessions conducted by mental health clinicians and complete a court-related anxiety measure before and after each session. Results from 175 participants are reported. Findings indicated significant decreases in court-related anxiety with large program effects. Challenges encountered by the program and future directions are discussed.  相似文献   
8.
9.
10.
Book reviews     
BODEN, MARGARET A. Artificial Intelligence and Natural Man. Hassocks : Harvester Press Limited, and New York: Basic Books Inc., 1977. Pp. ix+537. £613.50 hardback; £64.95 paperback. ISBN 0 85527 435 2.

ESTES, W. K. (Ed.). Handbook of Learning and Cognitive Processes. Vol. 6. Linguistics. Functions in Cognitive Theory. Hillsdale, N.J. : Erlbaum Associates. 1979. Pp. 331. £12.70. ISBN o 470 26311 3.

O'KEEFE, J. and NADEL, L. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. London: Oxford University Press. 1978. Pp. 543. £25.00. ISBN 0 12 524150 X.

FITZSIMONS, J. T. The Physiology of Thirst and Sodium Appetite. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1979. Pp. 572. £32.50. ISBN 0 521 22292 3.

MURRAY, F. B. and PIKULSKI, J. J. (Eds). The Acquisition of Reading. Baltimore: University Park Press. 1978. Pp. 178. £8.95. ISBN 0 8391 1281 5.

NEWTON, M. J., THOMAS, M. E. and RICHARDS, I. L. Readings in Dyslexia. Wisbech: Benrose Ltd. 1979. Pp. 203. £4.50. ISBN 09 0511 4124.

SENDERS, J. W., FISHER, D. F. and MONTY, R. A. (Eds). Eye Movements and Higher Psychological Processes. Hillsdale, N. J. : Erlbaum. 1978. Pp. 394. £19.95. ISBN 0 470 26489 6.

GRUBER, H. E. and VONECHE, J. J. (Eds). The Essential Piaget. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. 1977. Pp. 880. £12.95. ISBN 07100 87780.

SIEGEL, L. S. and BRAINERD, C. J. (Eds). Alternatives to Piaget. New York: Academic Press. 1978. Pp. 262. £11.70. ISBN 12 641950 7.

BRANSFORD, J. D. Human Cognition: Learning, Understanding and Remembering. Belmont, California: Wadsworth. 1979. Pp. xii+300. £11.85. ISBN 0 534 00699 X.

VON FIEANDT, K. and MOUSTGAARD, I. K. The Perceptual World. London: Academic Press. 1977. Pp. 680. £32.00. ISBN 0 12 725050 6.

BURTON, A. and RADFORD, J. (Ed.). Thinking in Perspective: Critical Essays in the Study of Thought Processes. London: Methuen. 1978. Pp. xxii+232. £3.95. ISBN 0 416 85840 6.

MAYER, R. E. Thinking and Problem Solving: An Introduction to Human Cognition and Leavning. Glenview, Illinois: Scott, Foresman and Company. 1977. Pp. viii+214. £4.95. ISBN 0 673 15055 0.

BATES, E. Language and Context: The Acquisition of Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press. 1976. Pp. 375. £14.50. ISBN 0 12 081550 8.

MILGHAM, N. W., KRAMES, L. and ALLOWAY, T. M. Food Aversion Learning. New York: Plenum Press. 1977. Pp. 263. £27. ISBN 0 306 31040 6.

O'CONNOR, N. and HERMELIN, B. Seeing and Hearing and Space and Time. London: Academic. 1978. Pp. 157+viii. £6.80. ISBN 0 12 524150 X.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号