全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1216篇 |
免费 | 18篇 |
专业分类
1234篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 12篇 |
2020年 | 13篇 |
2019年 | 25篇 |
2018年 | 29篇 |
2017年 | 26篇 |
2016年 | 36篇 |
2015年 | 28篇 |
2014年 | 19篇 |
2013年 | 104篇 |
2012年 | 40篇 |
2011年 | 50篇 |
2010年 | 22篇 |
2009年 | 21篇 |
2008年 | 32篇 |
2007年 | 38篇 |
2006年 | 31篇 |
2005年 | 38篇 |
2004年 | 30篇 |
2003年 | 42篇 |
2002年 | 26篇 |
2001年 | 19篇 |
2000年 | 38篇 |
1999年 | 26篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 19篇 |
1995年 | 23篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 16篇 |
1987年 | 23篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 17篇 |
1978年 | 23篇 |
1977年 | 16篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 20篇 |
1974年 | 18篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
1972年 | 11篇 |
1971年 | 10篇 |
1966年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有1234条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
D A Nelson 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1989,103(2):171-176
Estimate the amount of frequency variation that affected individual and species recognition by song in territorial male field sparrows with field playbacks of neighbors' songs lowered in frequency in 6 increments between 324 and 1296 Hz. Males gave strong responses to neighbors' songs that had been lowered by about 400 Hz, which is about 11% of the mean midfrequency in field sparrow song. Territorial responses were eliminated when neighbors' songs were lowered by more than about 900 Hz. Thus, there is a fairly narrow range of frequencies within which field sparrow songs can vary while functioning in communication. Ss' responses appear to be constrained by the normal range of song frequency variation within the species, but the range of variability within songs of individuals did not influence the degree of frequency shift required to render neighbors' songs unfamiliar. 相似文献
2.
3.
Kevin C. Mooney Robert Thompson James M. Nelson 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1987,15(1):67-73
The relationship between risk factors and the severity and type of childhood disorder, as measured by parent-completed Child Behavior Checklists, was examined for 768 children, ages 4–16, seen at a child mental health center. Regression analyses revealed no significant relationships between any combination of risk factors and the total number of behavior problems, internalizing, or externalizing scores for the entire group, males and females separately, or for the age and sex groupings of 6- to 11- and 12-to 16-yearolds. Chi-square tests revealed no relationship of profile type with any risk factor or with total number of risk factors. Implications for broad-based child assessment and risk factor research within a clinical population are discussed.We wish to thank Orin Bolstad, Linda Magnuson, and Julian Taplin for their assistance in this project. 相似文献
4.
The purpose of this study is to examine the interrelationships among coping statements, a self-report measure of anxiety and maladaptive overt behavior in a real-life stressful situation. Measures of these variables (interviews using TAT-like cards, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children, Behavior Profile Rating Scale) were taken from 23 adolescents prior to and during their dental visit. Results indicated that as disruptive "in-chair" overt behavior increased, so did the percentage of coping statements. In addition, as self-report levels of anxiety increased, the percentage of coping statements significantly decreased. Interestingly, there was no significant relationship between self-report levels of anxiety and disruptive in-chair behavior. It was also noted that the subjects' self-report levels of anxiety significantly decreased following their opportunity to verbalize their coping/noncoping statements via the TAT-like procedure. The implications of these interrelationships to cognitive-behavioral theory are discussed. 相似文献
5.
T O Nelson R J Leonesio R S Landwehr L Narens 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》1986,12(2):279-287
Distinctions are drawn between different predictors of an individual's memory performance, with emphasis on the notion of privileged access to idiosyncratic knowledge. Research is reported in which undergraduates attempted to recall the answers to general-information questions, then made feeling-of-knowing judgments on nonrecalled items, and subsequently had a criterion test (relearning, perceptual identification, or one of two versions of recognition). For predicting an individual's criterion performance, the individual's own feeling-of-knowing predictions were intermediate between two kinds of normative predictions: The individual's feeling-of-knowing predictions were more accurate than predictions derived from normative feeling-of-knowing ratings but were less accurate than predictions derived from base-rate item difficulty (normative probabilities of correct recall). Subsidiary analyses showed that factors other than unreliability are responsible for the partial inaccuracy of the individual's feeling of knowing. Ramifications are discussed for possible ways to improve the accuracy of an individual's feeling-of-knowing predictions. 相似文献
6.
Michael Bock 《Psychological research》1986,48(2):107-112
Summary How much does the recall of affectively positive, negative, and neutral words that differ in their emotional impact depend on the conceptual processing of these words? To answer this question one group of subjects were requested to rate the pronounceability of these words (formal orienting task), but were later tested for recall. Subsequently, they were asked to judge the intensity of the emotions aroused by the words. Another group of subjects received the latter, self-related task prior to the first exposure to the words. In both conditions the highly affect-arousing words were better recalled than the less emotional words. The affectively positive words were particularly well recalled, but only by those subjects involved in the formal orienting task. It is concluded that words are evaluated for their emotional content at an early stage of information processing as defined by Craik and Lockhart (1972).Extended version of an earlier paper delivered at the 34. Kongreß der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie in Vienna, 1984 相似文献
7.
Nelson Cowan 《Behavior research methods》1986,18(5):434-446
In language-related psychological research, it is often necessary to search for sets of words with certain well-controlled phonological properties. To aid in searches of this kind, this paper presents a matrix of consonant-cluster-free, monosyllabic English words that are classified according to their phonemes. The matrix is of considerable use in the construction of experimental stimuli. Its applications are discussed. 相似文献
8.
Robin B. Jarrett Rosemery O. Nelson 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1984,6(2):131-145
This study examined the reliability and reactivity of participant observation. Twelve couples engaged in two discussions in a laboratory, one on a conflict topic and one on a consensus topic. For the eight couples in the experimental sequences, a baseline discussion was followed by participant observation in which the husband recorded his wife's use of I while continuing to converse. The four couples in the control sequences conversed without participant observation. Compared to observations made by a trained observer, the husbands were unreliable observers, underestimating their wives' use of I. Participant observation was reactive (P<.01), with more wives decreasing their use of I in the experimental sequences than in the control sequences (p<.094). The topic of discussion did not differentially influence reliability or reactivity.This research was conducted as the first author's independent doctoral research project. We extend gratitude to the Research Council of the University of North Carolina at Greensboro for Grant 0-2-110-218-XXXXX-7548; the Statistical Consulting Center of the University of North Carolina at Greensboro; two anonymous reviewers; and our research assistants, Suzanne Brannon, Ben Hardie, Kathy Lindamood, Amy Mitchem, Regina Pierce, and Elga Wulfert. Portions of this paper were presented at the meeting of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Washington, D.C., December 1983. 相似文献
9.
10.
To test the hypothesized immaturity of juvenile delinquents' moral reasoning, the results of 15 studies of the moral reasoning of juvenile delinquents were integrated quantitatively using meta-analysis. Hedges and Olkin (1985) methods were used to (a) compute effect sizes, (b) test the homogeneity of the obtained effect sizes, and (c) test the statistical significance of the pooled mean effect size. The results supported the hypothesis that the moral reasoning of juvenile delinquents is immature. It was concluded that several other issues are in need of investigation. 相似文献