首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   7874篇
  免费   195篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2023年   43篇
  2022年   38篇
  2021年   70篇
  2020年   86篇
  2019年   136篇
  2018年   264篇
  2017年   249篇
  2016年   327篇
  2015年   210篇
  2014年   147篇
  2013年   471篇
  2012年   469篇
  2011年   472篇
  2010年   149篇
  2009年   119篇
  2008年   190篇
  2007年   182篇
  2006年   145篇
  2005年   1394篇
  2004年   736篇
  2003年   523篇
  2002年   223篇
  2001年   85篇
  2000年   72篇
  1999年   67篇
  1998年   35篇
  1997年   23篇
  1996年   30篇
  1993年   26篇
  1992年   49篇
  1991年   69篇
  1990年   91篇
  1989年   64篇
  1988年   52篇
  1987年   61篇
  1986年   61篇
  1985年   49篇
  1984年   50篇
  1983年   38篇
  1979年   44篇
  1978年   22篇
  1975年   32篇
  1974年   41篇
  1973年   37篇
  1972年   29篇
  1971年   32篇
  1969年   23篇
  1968年   23篇
  1967年   23篇
  1966年   22篇
排序方式: 共有8071条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
F R?tzer 《Psyche》1987,41(8):717-725
  相似文献   
2.
Adult age differences in the consistency effect were examined in 3 experiments. The consistency effect refers to items inconsistent with expectations being better remembered than items consistent with expectations. Younger and older adults walked into an office room and viewed objects that varied in their consistency with expectation. Immediate and delayed recognition tests on item information (i.e., distractors were defined by their semantic identity) revealed that both age groups recognized unexpected items better than expected items. However, when recognition of token information was requested (i.e., distractors were defined by their physical appearance), younger adults, in contrast to older adults, exhibited consistency effects. Also, under divided attention, young adults revealed the same pattern of data as did elderly adults under full attention. The results are discussed in terms of capacity-related differences in distinctive encoding.  相似文献   
3.
Abstract: One claim about I , regularly made and almost universally endorsed, is that uses of the term are logically guaranteed to refer successfully (if they refer at all). The claim is only rarely formulated perspicuously or argued for. Such obscurity helps disguise the fact that those who profess to advance the claim actually turn out to support not a logical guarantee at all but merely high security through fortunate coincidence. This is not surprising. For we have no good reason to accept the claim – granted, any use of I is apt to refer successfully; but that can be explained by pragmatic features of its use. And we have some reason to reject the claim – it is notoriously difficult to see how genuine reference and guaranteed success do not exclude each other when considered as properties of the logic of any term.  相似文献   
4.
5.
Several studies have illuminated how processing manual action verbs (MaVs) affects the programming or execution of concurrent hand movements. Here, to circumvent key confounds in extant designs, we conducted the first assessment of motor–language integration during handwriting—a task in which linguistic and motoric processes are co‐substantiated. Participants copied MaVs, non‐manual action verbs, and non‐action verbs as we collected measures of motor programming and motor execution. Programming latencies were similar across conditions, but execution was faster for MaVs than for the other categories, regardless of whether word meanings were accessed implicitly or explicitly. In line with the Hand‐Action‐Network Dynamic Language Embodiment (HANDLE) model, such findings suggest that effector‐congruent verbs can prime manual movements even during highly automatized tasks in which motoric and verbal processes are naturally intertwined. Our paradigm opens new avenues for fine‐grained explorations of embodied language processes.  相似文献   
6.
7.
8.
It was predicted that trained observers would detect deception more accurately than untrained observers. More specifically, it was predicted that the highest deception detection accuracy would be found among trained observers judging the veracity of low self-monitors and unrehearsed liars, whereas the lowest detection accuracy would be found among untrained observers judging the veracity of high self-monitors and rehearsed deceivers. It also was hypothesized that the discrepancy between observers‘actual ability to detect deception and their certainty in the accuracy of their judgments would be smaller for trained observers than for untrained observers. Observers trained to detect deception used six behavioral cues based on research by deTurck and Miller (1985): (a) message duration, (b) response latency, (c) adaptors, (d) pauses, (e) nonfluencies, and (f) hand gestures. Results confirmed both hypotheses.  相似文献   
9.
10.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号