首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2058篇
  免费   114篇
  2023年   16篇
  2022年   16篇
  2021年   30篇
  2020年   51篇
  2019年   50篇
  2018年   92篇
  2017年   91篇
  2016年   77篇
  2015年   53篇
  2014年   62篇
  2013年   276篇
  2012年   114篇
  2011年   87篇
  2010年   70篇
  2009年   75篇
  2008年   76篇
  2007年   92篇
  2006年   77篇
  2005年   54篇
  2004年   52篇
  2003年   67篇
  2002年   57篇
  2001年   29篇
  2000年   44篇
  1999年   35篇
  1998年   27篇
  1997年   13篇
  1996年   16篇
  1995年   13篇
  1994年   14篇
  1993年   14篇
  1992年   14篇
  1990年   19篇
  1989年   13篇
  1988年   16篇
  1987年   13篇
  1986年   12篇
  1985年   16篇
  1984年   18篇
  1983年   11篇
  1982年   9篇
  1981年   9篇
  1976年   10篇
  1975年   10篇
  1973年   12篇
  1972年   9篇
  1971年   9篇
  1970年   9篇
  1951年   11篇
  1950年   7篇
排序方式: 共有2172条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Drawing on terror management theory (TMT), we discuss the psychological motivations that shape personality at two levels: the characteristically human personality common to us all and the individual differences that distinguish some people from others. TMT posits that the motivation to protect the self against deep-rooted fears about mortality drives people to maintain meaningful, reliable conceptions of reality and positive evaluations of themselves, two broad tendencies that form the foundation of every person’s personality. We review studies showing that mortality reminders increase efforts to bolster cultural sources of meaning and self-esteem in similar ways across individuals and cultures. TMT also posits that individual differences in personality partly reflect the different sources of meaning and self-esteem that people invest in to assuage mortality fears. We review evidence that individual differences predict the degree and direction of people’s defensive responses to mortality reminders. Directions for future research are discussed.  相似文献   
2.
Two experiments investigated the interpretation of anaphoric noun phrases, and in particular those thatcould only be linked to their antecedents via knowledge-based inferences. The first experiment showed that much of the inferential processing was carried out as the anaphoric noun phrase was read, although there was some indication that inferential processing continued to the end of the clause. The second experiment attempted to establish why anaphoric noun phrases that are more specific than their antecedents cause problems. It showed that the difficulty did not lie in adding the extra information carried by the anaphor to the representation of the referent. Rather, we suggest, putting extra information in the anaphoric noun phrase disrupts the process of linking that noun phrase to its antecedent.  相似文献   
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
Clinical judgments on the treatability and prognosis of mentally disordered offeenders (MDO) may strongly influence legal dispositions and the availability of treatment resources. This study examined 1,238 discharge summaries for MDOs referred for court assessments. Psychiatrists evidenced marked variability in how often they recommended treatment and how frequently they judged patients to have a poor prognosis. Two logit analyses suggested that diagnosis and consultation by other clinicians were associated with (a) treatment recommendations (i.e., Axis I diagnosis and social work consultations) and (b) prognosis (i.e., Axis II diagnosis and psychology consultations).  相似文献   
8.
9.
The heterosexual nature of the HIV epidemic in Lothian is such that a number (50 to date) of babies have been born to women who have HIV. Some have needed social work services such as Children Centre placement, foster parents or adopters. Since this work began in 1986, our understanding of the transmission and manifestation of HIV infection in children has grown. However, it is still not possible until children reach the age of two to be certain whether or not they have been infected, or what the outlook is for them. Even then there are some further areas of uncertainty. We have developed a programme of preparation, training and support which has enabled a number of families to live with this uncertainly in offering placements to children at risk of HIV infection.  相似文献   
10.
Hardyck, Tzeng, and Wang (1978, Brain and Language, 5, 56–71) hypothesized that ample repetition of a small number of stimuli is required in order to obtain VHF differences in tachistoscopic tasks. Four experiments, with varied levels of repetition, were conducted to test this hypothesis. Three experiments utilized the general task of object-picture naming and one utilized a word-naming task. Naming latencies constituted the dependent measure. The results demonstrate that for the object-naming paradigm repetition is required for RVF superiority to emerge. Repetition was found to be unnecessary for RVF superiority in the word-naming paradigm, with repetition actually reducing RVF superiority. Experiment I suggested the possibility that RVF superiority developed for the second half of the trials as a function of practice or hemispheric activation, regardless of repetition level. Subsequent experiments, better designed to assess this possibility, clearly refuted it. It was concluded that the effect of repetition depends on the processing requirements of the task. We propose that, for tasks which can be processed efficiently by one hemisphere, the effect of repetition will be to reduce VHF asymmetries; but tasks requiring substantial processing by both hemispheres will show shifts to RVF superiority as a function of repetition.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号