首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1833篇
  免费   73篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2021年   15篇
  2020年   38篇
  2019年   30篇
  2018年   47篇
  2017年   49篇
  2016年   52篇
  2015年   23篇
  2014年   44篇
  2013年   231篇
  2012年   56篇
  2011年   50篇
  2010年   42篇
  2009年   48篇
  2008年   64篇
  2007年   65篇
  2006年   52篇
  2005年   61篇
  2004年   55篇
  2003年   55篇
  2002年   51篇
  2001年   36篇
  2000年   28篇
  1999年   26篇
  1998年   27篇
  1997年   24篇
  1996年   21篇
  1995年   24篇
  1994年   19篇
  1993年   17篇
  1992年   35篇
  1991年   28篇
  1990年   20篇
  1987年   26篇
  1986年   15篇
  1985年   20篇
  1984年   15篇
  1983年   18篇
  1982年   25篇
  1981年   26篇
  1980年   14篇
  1979年   19篇
  1978年   17篇
  1977年   14篇
  1975年   15篇
  1974年   20篇
  1973年   22篇
  1972年   15篇
  1971年   15篇
  1970年   15篇
  1969年   15篇
排序方式: 共有1907条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
4.
Developing readers often make anagrammatical errors (e.g. misreading pirates as parties), suggesting they use letter position flexibly during word recognition. However, while it is widely assumed that the occurrence of these errors decreases with increases in reading skill, empirical evidence to support this distinction is lacking. Accordingly, we compared the performance of developing child readers (aged 8–10 years) against the end‐state performance of skilled adult readers in a timed naming task, employing anagrams used previously in this area of research. Moreover, to explore the use of letter position by developing readers and skilled adult readers more fully, we used anagrams which, to form another word, required letter transpositions over only interior letter positions, or both interior and exterior letter positions. The patterns of effects across these two anagram types for the two groups of readers were very similar. In particular, both groups showed similarly slowed response times (and developing readers increased errors) for anagrams requiring only interior letter transpositions but not for anagrams that required exterior letter transpositions. This similarity in the naming performance of developing readers and skilled adult readers suggests that the end‐state skilled use of letter position is established earlier during reading development than is widely assumed.  相似文献   
5.
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
Verbal expressions of probability are used in daily conversations, physician-physician and physician-patient communications, and questionnaire and interview responses. To assess the degree of agreement among English-speaking Australian adults in allocating numerical probabilities to these verbal expressions of probability, 966 interviewees provided estimates for 60 isolated expressions of probability and a sequence of seven items placed in a sequence. Means and median scores appeared to be consistent with common sense and with findings from other countries. Mirror-imaged terms were neither symmetrical nor equidistant, with the means and medians for the positive terms being closer to the mid-points of the scale than the means and medians of the negative terms. Items in a sequence of probability terms showed greater symmetry and less variability than isolated expressions. For most items, there was an unacceptably high level of within-subject and between-subject variability. Although subjects with higher levels of education and/ or mathematics education showed less variability, these factors accounted for very little of the variance. The greater variability in Australian results relative to those reported elsewhere was partly attributed to the use of interviews rather than questionnaires. There were no particular stems that yielded greater consistency. It was concluded that the use of these expressions leads to very imprecise communication.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号