首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2503篇
  免费   108篇
  2023年   15篇
  2022年   13篇
  2021年   30篇
  2020年   42篇
  2019年   50篇
  2018年   71篇
  2017年   83篇
  2016年   93篇
  2015年   69篇
  2014年   70篇
  2013年   309篇
  2012年   143篇
  2011年   138篇
  2010年   104篇
  2009年   72篇
  2008年   107篇
  2007年   138篇
  2006年   102篇
  2005年   78篇
  2004年   86篇
  2003年   71篇
  2002年   70篇
  2001年   41篇
  2000年   33篇
  1999年   20篇
  1998年   26篇
  1997年   30篇
  1996年   30篇
  1995年   22篇
  1994年   22篇
  1993年   24篇
  1992年   34篇
  1991年   20篇
  1990年   24篇
  1989年   25篇
  1988年   22篇
  1987年   20篇
  1985年   12篇
  1984年   14篇
  1983年   13篇
  1982年   24篇
  1981年   16篇
  1980年   13篇
  1978年   20篇
  1977年   8篇
  1976年   12篇
  1974年   15篇
  1971年   9篇
  1968年   10篇
  1966年   8篇
排序方式: 共有2611条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
1.
2.
3.
Whether people compete or cooperate with each other has consequences for their own performance and that of organizations. To explain why people compete or cooperate, previous research has focused on two main factors: situational outcome structures and personality types. Here, we propose that—above and beyond these two factors—situational cues, such as the format in which people receive feedback, strongly affect whether they act competitively, cooperatively, or individualistically. Results of a laboratory experiment support our theorizing: After receiving ranking feedback, both students and experienced managers treated group situations with cooperative outcome structures as competitive and were in consequence willing to forgo guaranteed financial gains to pursue a—financially irrelevant—better rank. Conversely, in dilemma situations, feedback based on the joint group outcome led to more cooperation than ranking feedback. Our study contributes to research on competition, cooperation, interdependence theory, forced ranking, and the design of information environments.  相似文献   
4.
5.
6.
Three experiments tested the hypothesis that people can execute focused searches of semantic cases when answering questions about complex facts, such as “the teacher watered the peas, the corn, and the lettuce with the hose.” In Experiment 1, answer time varied mainly with the number of concepts in the relevant or focused case, supporting the hypothesis. Experiment 2 indicated that the irrelevant case undergoes some processing. Experiment 3 confirmed that search in Experiments 1 and 2 focused on semantic cases rather than on taxonomic categories. It is proposed that focused memory search has a high probability of retrieving the relevant case and a low probability of retrieving the irrelevant case. In the latter event, the irrelevant concepts receive full processing.  相似文献   
7.
8.
The Psychological Record - The acceptability of labels for behavioral response-reduction procedures and the users of those procedures was evaluated. Introductory psychology students read a lesson...  相似文献   
9.
This paper suggests a method to supplant missing categorical data by reasonable replacements. These replacements will maximize the consistency of the completed data as measured by Guttman's squared correlation ratio. The text outlines a solution of the optimization problem, describes relationships with the relevant psychometric theory, and studies some properties of the method in detail. The main result is that the average correlation should be at least 0.50 before the method becomes practical. At that point, the technique gives reasonable results up to 10–15% missing data.We thank Anneke Bloemhoff of NIPG-TNO for compiling and making the Dutch Life Style Survey data available to use, and Chantal Houée and Thérèse Bardaine, IUT, Vannes, France, exchange students under the COMETT program of the EC, for computational assistance. We also thank Donald Rubin, the Editors and several anonymous reviewers for constructive suggestions.  相似文献   
10.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号