首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   14294篇
  免费   373篇
  国内免费   3篇
  2020年   130篇
  2019年   169篇
  2018年   302篇
  2017年   322篇
  2016年   422篇
  2015年   276篇
  2014年   267篇
  2013年   1141篇
  2012年   650篇
  2011年   619篇
  2010年   287篇
  2009年   266篇
  2008年   391篇
  2007年   375篇
  2006年   359篇
  2005年   1579篇
  2004年   940篇
  2003年   713篇
  2002年   467篇
  2001年   174篇
  2000年   148篇
  1999年   180篇
  1998年   162篇
  1997年   143篇
  1996年   133篇
  1995年   147篇
  1994年   135篇
  1993年   117篇
  1992年   158篇
  1991年   155篇
  1990年   162篇
  1989年   145篇
  1988年   103篇
  1987年   120篇
  1986年   120篇
  1985年   124篇
  1984年   144篇
  1983年   151篇
  1982年   127篇
  1981年   158篇
  1980年   121篇
  1979年   162篇
  1978年   149篇
  1977年   136篇
  1976年   129篇
  1975年   145篇
  1974年   150篇
  1973年   106篇
  1972年   104篇
  1969年   86篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
F R?tzer 《Psyche》1987,41(8):717-725
  相似文献   
3.
4.
Adult age differences in the consistency effect were examined in 3 experiments. The consistency effect refers to items inconsistent with expectations being better remembered than items consistent with expectations. Younger and older adults walked into an office room and viewed objects that varied in their consistency with expectation. Immediate and delayed recognition tests on item information (i.e., distractors were defined by their semantic identity) revealed that both age groups recognized unexpected items better than expected items. However, when recognition of token information was requested (i.e., distractors were defined by their physical appearance), younger adults, in contrast to older adults, exhibited consistency effects. Also, under divided attention, young adults revealed the same pattern of data as did elderly adults under full attention. The results are discussed in terms of capacity-related differences in distinctive encoding.  相似文献   
5.
James Cargile 《Topoi》2003,22(2):143-149
  相似文献   
6.
7.
8.
The authors used recursive partitioning methods to identify combinations of baseline characteristics that predict 2-year physical activity success in each of 3 physical activity interventions delivered in the multisite Activity Counseling Trial. The sample consisted of 874 initially sedentary primary care patients, ages 35-75 years, who were at risk for cardiovascular disease. Predictors of 2-year success were specific to each intervention and represented a range of domains, including physiological, demographic, psychosocial, health-related, and environmental variables. The results indicate how specific patient subgroups (e.g., obese, unfit individuals; high-income individuals in stable health) may respond differently to varying levels and amounts of professional assistance and support. The methods used provide a practical first step toward identifying clinically meaningful patient subgroups for further systematic investigation.  相似文献   
9.
One possible explanation for the association between Cook-Medley Hostility Scale (Ho Scale; W. W. Cook & D. M. Medley, 1954) scores and premature coronary artery disease (CAD) morbidity and mortality is that hostile persons also have elevations on CAD risk factors. Meta-analyses with fixed and random-effects models were used to evaluate the relationship between Ho Scale scores and CAD risk factors in the empirical literature. Ho Scale scores were significantly related to body mass index, waist-to-hip ratio, insulin resistance, lipid ratio, triglycerides, glucose, socioeconomic status (SES), alcohol consumption, and smoking. Although there was also heterogeneity among study outcomes, the results of conservative random effects models provide confidence in the obtained relationships. On the basis of available evidence, researchers might give attention to obesity, insulin resistance, damaging health behaviors, and SES as potential contributing factors in understanding the association between Ho Scale scores and CAD.  相似文献   
10.
Human communication is thoroughly context bound. We present two experiments investigating the importance of the shared context, that is, the amount of knowledge two interlocutors have in common, for the successful emergence and use of novel conventions. Using a referential communication task where black‐and‐white pictorial symbols are used to convey colors, pairs of participants build shared conventions peculiar to their dyad without experimenter feedback, relying purely on ostensive‐inferential communication. Both experiments demonstrate that access to the visual context promotes more successful communication. Importantly, success improves cumulatively, supporting the view that pairs establish conventional ways of using the symbols to communicate. Furthermore, Experiment 2 suggests that dyads with access to the visual context successfully adapt the conventions built for one color space to another color space, unlike dyads lacking it. In linking experimental pragmatics with language evolution, the study illustrates the benefits of exploring the emergence of linguistic conventions using an ostensive‐inferential model of communication.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号