全文获取类型
收费全文 | 18930篇 |
免费 | 388篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
19321篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 156篇 |
2020年 | 254篇 |
2019年 | 236篇 |
2018年 | 377篇 |
2017年 | 349篇 |
2016年 | 387篇 |
2015年 | 303篇 |
2014年 | 363篇 |
2013年 | 1436篇 |
2012年 | 662篇 |
2011年 | 575篇 |
2010年 | 415篇 |
2009年 | 384篇 |
2008年 | 525篇 |
2007年 | 557篇 |
2006年 | 446篇 |
2005年 | 431篇 |
2004年 | 377篇 |
2003年 | 325篇 |
2002年 | 358篇 |
2001年 | 731篇 |
2000年 | 724篇 |
1999年 | 495篇 |
1998年 | 190篇 |
1997年 | 187篇 |
1996年 | 178篇 |
1992年 | 382篇 |
1991年 | 354篇 |
1990年 | 382篇 |
1989年 | 329篇 |
1988年 | 303篇 |
1987年 | 310篇 |
1986年 | 279篇 |
1985年 | 278篇 |
1984年 | 264篇 |
1983年 | 220篇 |
1982年 | 165篇 |
1979年 | 246篇 |
1978年 | 183篇 |
1977年 | 159篇 |
1975年 | 211篇 |
1974年 | 252篇 |
1973年 | 249篇 |
1972年 | 198篇 |
1971年 | 196篇 |
1970年 | 169篇 |
1969年 | 185篇 |
1968年 | 226篇 |
1967年 | 202篇 |
1966年 | 165篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
The individual deciding on a nuclear weapons production policy is faced with a dilemma in which a choice must be made to support one of several conflicting defense policies. In order to investigate the social-psychological variables that are related to armament policy decision making, a telephone survey was conducted in the City of St. Louis, Missouri. Adult subjects (N= 110) responded to questions concerned with cognitive, attitudinal, and emotional responses to nuclear arms. Participants were also asked to choose a weapons production policy for the United States under conditions of continued and decreased Soviet nuclear arms production. Results indicated that respondents decisions about nuclear weapons production were related to their support for deterrence, the availability of their nuclear-related images, their emotional response to nuclear war, their attributions of responsibility for the prevention of nuclear war, and their political party affiliation. Distinctions between antinuclear decision-makers and pronuclear decision-makers are discussed. 相似文献
2.
Personality in adulthood: a six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO Personality Inventory 总被引:20,自引:0,他引:20
Previous longitudinal studies of personality in adulthood have been limited in the range of traits examined, have chiefly made use of self-reports, and have frequently included only men. In this study, self-reports (N = 983) and spouse ratings (N = 167) were gathered on the NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1985b), which measures all five of the major dimensions of normal personality. Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses on data from men and women aged 21 to 96 years showed evidence of small declines in Activity, Positive Emotions, and openness to Actions that might be attributed to maturation, but none of these effects was replicated in sequential analyses. The 20 other scales examined showed no consistent pattern of maturational effects. In contrast, retest stability was quite high for all five dimensions in self-reports and for the three dimensions measured at both times in spouse ratings. Comparable levels of stability were seen for men and women and for younger and older subjects. The data support the position that personality is stable after age 30. 相似文献
3.
A "change-of-standard" perspective on the relations among context, judgment, and memory 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
We conducted three studies that tested a "change-of-standard" perspective on the relations among context, judgment, and recall. Each study consisted of two or three sessions held a few days apart. All subjects read about the sentencing decisions of one or two target trial judges and of six nontarget trial judges who consistently gave either higher or lower sentences than the target judge(s). Each study varied both the standard that was available when subjects initially judged the sentencing decisions of a target judge and the standard available when subjects subsequently recalled those decisions. To accomplish this, we varied the context of judgment, the timing of judgment, and the overall category norm for trial judges' sentencing decisions that was available at recall. We found that although subjects had been exposed to the same target information and had initially judged it in the same way, their recall of the information was different depending on whether and how a change-of-standard had occurred between judgment and recall. Unique predictions of the change-of-standard perspective were confirmed that could not be accounted for in terms of other types of context effects on judgment and memory. 相似文献
4.
J T Cacioppo J S Martzke R E Petty L G Tassinary 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,54(4):592-604
Previous research has demonstrated that mild negative emotional imagery and unpleasant sensory stimuli lead to greater electromyographic activity over the brow muscle region than mild positive imagery and stimuli, even in the absence of significant changes in visceral and general facial EMG activity. Previous research has not addressed whether electromyographic responses over the brow region are a sensitive and specific index of emotions, however, since a multiplicity of events lead to changes in brow activity. In this research, facial electromyographic and audiovisual recordings were obtained while individuals were interviewed about themselves. Afterwards, individuals were asked to describe what they had been thinking of during specific segments of the interview marked by distinctive electromyographic responses over the brow region in the context of ongoing but stable levels of activity elsewhere in the face. The results are interpreted in terms of a continuous flow hypothesis of affect-laden information processing. 相似文献
5.
6.
7.
Wolfgang Hell Gerd Gigerenzer Siegfried Gauggel Maria Mall Michael Müller 《Memory & cognition》1988,16(6):533-538
If subjects are asked to recollect a former response after having been informed about the correct response, their recollection tends to approach the correct response. This effect has been termedhindsight bias. We studied hindsight bias in an experiment requiring numerical responses to almanac-type questions for physical quantities. We varied (1) the time at which the correct information was provided, (2) the encoding of the original responses by asking/not asking subjects to give a reason for the respective response, and (3) the motivation to recall correctly. We found that hindsight is less biased if reasons are given and if the correct information is provided at an earlier time. Motivation had only interactive effects: (1) With high motivation to recall correctly, the time the correct information was provided had no influence. (2) With reasons given, the variation of motivation showed no effect. These results rule out purely motivational and purely automatic explanations. 相似文献
8.
9.
E Schier T Yecunnoamlack T Tegegne 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1988,40(8):490-499
Of 1240 first-time consultations in the neuropsychiatric clinic of the Gondar College of Medical Sciences in an eleven-month period during 1985 and 1986, 152 patients came seeking relief from seizures. This paper deals with the origins and background of diagnostic, differential diagnostic, and therapeutic problems, and with problems of social rehabilitation. While in such a tradition-bound society certain hypotheses continue to have an unfavorable effect, there are unmistakable signs of positive change against a background of rapid social change. Conclusions are drawn for the better management of epileptic patients in developing countries. 相似文献
10.