首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   7460篇
  免费   160篇
  国内免费   9篇
  2023年   40篇
  2022年   32篇
  2021年   56篇
  2020年   68篇
  2019年   112篇
  2018年   236篇
  2017年   219篇
  2016年   315篇
  2015年   188篇
  2014年   129篇
  2013年   410篇
  2012年   437篇
  2011年   433篇
  2010年   130篇
  2009年   109篇
  2008年   172篇
  2007年   160篇
  2006年   137篇
  2005年   1382篇
  2004年   732篇
  2003年   524篇
  2002年   213篇
  2001年   80篇
  2000年   68篇
  1999年   62篇
  1998年   33篇
  1997年   23篇
  1996年   28篇
  1995年   23篇
  1993年   25篇
  1992年   47篇
  1991年   69篇
  1990年   91篇
  1989年   63篇
  1988年   53篇
  1987年   62篇
  1986年   61篇
  1985年   49篇
  1984年   48篇
  1983年   38篇
  1979年   44篇
  1975年   32篇
  1974年   40篇
  1973年   37篇
  1972年   28篇
  1971年   32篇
  1969年   23篇
  1968年   23篇
  1967年   23篇
  1966年   22篇
排序方式: 共有7629条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
F R?tzer 《Psyche》1987,41(8):717-725
  相似文献   
2.
Adult age differences in the consistency effect were examined in 3 experiments. The consistency effect refers to items inconsistent with expectations being better remembered than items consistent with expectations. Younger and older adults walked into an office room and viewed objects that varied in their consistency with expectation. Immediate and delayed recognition tests on item information (i.e., distractors were defined by their semantic identity) revealed that both age groups recognized unexpected items better than expected items. However, when recognition of token information was requested (i.e., distractors were defined by their physical appearance), younger adults, in contrast to older adults, exhibited consistency effects. Also, under divided attention, young adults revealed the same pattern of data as did elderly adults under full attention. The results are discussed in terms of capacity-related differences in distinctive encoding.  相似文献   
3.
Abstract: One claim about I , regularly made and almost universally endorsed, is that uses of the term are logically guaranteed to refer successfully (if they refer at all). The claim is only rarely formulated perspicuously or argued for. Such obscurity helps disguise the fact that those who profess to advance the claim actually turn out to support not a logical guarantee at all but merely high security through fortunate coincidence. This is not surprising. For we have no good reason to accept the claim – granted, any use of I is apt to refer successfully; but that can be explained by pragmatic features of its use. And we have some reason to reject the claim – it is notoriously difficult to see how genuine reference and guaranteed success do not exclude each other when considered as properties of the logic of any term.  相似文献   
4.
5.
6.
7.
It was predicted that trained observers would detect deception more accurately than untrained observers. More specifically, it was predicted that the highest deception detection accuracy would be found among trained observers judging the veracity of low self-monitors and unrehearsed liars, whereas the lowest detection accuracy would be found among untrained observers judging the veracity of high self-monitors and rehearsed deceivers. It also was hypothesized that the discrepancy between observers‘actual ability to detect deception and their certainty in the accuracy of their judgments would be smaller for trained observers than for untrained observers. Observers trained to detect deception used six behavioral cues based on research by deTurck and Miller (1985): (a) message duration, (b) response latency, (c) adaptors, (d) pauses, (e) nonfluencies, and (f) hand gestures. Results confirmed both hypotheses.  相似文献   
8.
9.
10.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号