首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   444篇
  免费   15篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2023年   3篇
  2021年   5篇
  2018年   15篇
  2017年   14篇
  2016年   8篇
  2015年   7篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   56篇
  2012年   18篇
  2011年   24篇
  2010年   12篇
  2009年   9篇
  2008年   10篇
  2007年   17篇
  2006年   8篇
  2005年   11篇
  2004年   11篇
  2003年   14篇
  2002年   18篇
  2001年   10篇
  2000年   6篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   5篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   4篇
  1991年   4篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   3篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   5篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   8篇
  1983年   4篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   6篇
  1978年   6篇
  1977年   6篇
  1974年   10篇
  1973年   7篇
  1972年   6篇
  1971年   8篇
  1970年   3篇
  1968年   4篇
  1967年   8篇
  1966年   5篇
  1965年   3篇
  1933年   2篇
  1925年   3篇
  1922年   2篇
  1920年   2篇
排序方式: 共有460条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Preface     
Kurt H. Wolff 《Human Studies》1984,7(3-4):105-106
  相似文献   
2.
3.
4.
Phenomena of language teaching and learning in the course of verbal interactions between one mother and her daughter were analyzed. The daughter was between 18 and 27 months old during the recordings, and her utterance length ranged from 1,5 to 4,0 morphemes. Twenty hours of interactions were analyzed and the emphasis was placed upon sequential aspects and mathemagenic features of the conversations. It was demonstrated that the mother performs much analytic, synthetic, abstracting, and word-class-defining work during these verbal interactions. These maternal instructional activities seem to lead not only to the child's learning of language rules but also to her employment of the abstracting, analytical, and synthetic methodology. It is concluded that the main explanatory focus has to be on the mother in the attempts to explain language transmission and acquisition. Basic similarities to other instructional/skill training situations are suggested. Neither extraordinary complex cognitive nor innate linguistic capacities need to be assumed to explain the phenomena in question.  相似文献   
5.
Summary Subjects were shown eight-letter pseudowords tachistoscopically and were asked to report as many letters as possible. The pseudowords, examples of either first- or fourth-order approximation to English, were printed in normal or reversed spelling and were presented in either normal of mirror-imaged letters. Finally subjects were either free to report in any order they wished, or they were instructed to report from left to right or from right to left. With normal letters, the familiarity effect was controlled by the spelling direction, not by the direction of report or the match between direction of report and spelling direction. With mirrored letters, however, order of approximation to English had no effect. The results contradict two existing theories: Mewhort's theory claims that the familiarity of fourth-order materials aids processing at a postcategorical level; it has trouble explaining why the familiarity effect disappeared with mirrored letters and normal spelling. Wolff's theory claims that familiarity aids processing at the feature level; it has trouble explaining why the familiarity effect disappeared with mirrored letters and reversed spelling.  相似文献   
6.
7.
Skin conductance and heart rate were continuously recorded in 8 subjects repeatedly exposed to a perceptual-conflict test and to auditory signals. (1) Both complex and simple stimuli gave rise to a pronounced increase in skin conductance, the magnitude of response decreasing with number of repetitions. For both types of stimuli a linear relationship was obtained when GSR, measured as the change in log conductance, was plotted against the logarithm of the stimulus number. (2) Each presentation of the conflict test increased heart rate, magnitude of change decreasing approximately linearly with number of trials.  相似文献   
8.
9.
10.
Rate variables and automatized naming in developmental dyslexia   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
The rate variable in rapid automatized naming (RAN) was investigated in 50 adolescent and 40 adult students with developmental dyslexia, in matched normal controls, and in learning-disabled students without reading difficulties. Visual stimuli depicting familiar colors and common objects were presented in isolation at three film speeds and three exposure times. Film speed and exposure time contributed as independent variables to error rate; and dyslexic subjects of both age groups made significantly more naming errors than controls. Dyslexic subjects also responded with longer naming latencies than controls when the same RAN stimuli were presented in a continuous sequential mode as a matrix of rows and columns. Naming latencies in the sequential presentation were highly correlated with naming errors in the film version. The implications of reduced naming rates for nongraphological stimuli in developmental dyslexia are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号