全文获取类型
收费全文 | 637篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
648篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 27篇 |
2016年 | 15篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 52篇 |
2012年 | 23篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 21篇 |
2009年 | 20篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 16篇 |
2006年 | 15篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 25篇 |
2003年 | 20篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有648条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
The multiple and long-term effects of functional communication training relative to a common reductive procedure (time-out from positive reinforcement) were evaluated. Twelve children participated in a functional analysis of their challenging behaviors (Study 1), which implicated adult attention as a maintaining variable. The children were then matched for chronological age, mental age, and language age and assigned to two groups. One group received functional communication training as an intervention for their challenging behavior, and the second group received time-out as a contrast. Both interventions were initially successful (Study 2), but durable results were achieved only with the group that received functional communication training across different stimulus conditions (Study 3). Students whose challenging behaviors were previously reduced with time-out resumed these behaviors in the presence of naive teachers unaware of the children's intervention history. The value of teaching communicative responses to promote maintenance is discussed as it relates to the concept of functional equivalence. 相似文献
2.
3.
4.
5.
Beilock SL Jellison WA Rydell RJ McConnell AR Carr TH 《Personality & social psychology bulletin》2006,32(8):1059-1071
Recent work suggests that stereotype threat (ST) harms performance by reducing available working memory capacity. Is this the only mechanism by which ST can occur? Three experiments examined ST's impact on expert golf putting, which is not harmed when working memory is reduced but is hurt when attention is allocated to proceduralized processes that normally run outside working memory. Experiment 1 showed that well learned golf putting is susceptible to ST. Experiments 2 and 3 demonstrated that giving expert golfers a secondary task eliminates ST-induced impairment. Distracting attention away from the stereotype-related behavior eliminates the harmful impact of negative stereotype activation. These results are consistent with explicit monitoring theories of choking under pressure, which suggest that performance degradation can occur when too much attention is allocated to processes that usually run more automatically. Thus, ST alters information processing in multiple ways, inducing performance decrements for different reasons in different tasks. 相似文献
6.
Paul Henry Carr 《Zygon》2001,36(2):255-259
Paul Tillich noted the emergence of science by "demythologization" from its original unity with religion in antiquity. Demythologization can lead to conflict with accepted paradigms and therefore requires the "courage to create," as exemplified by Galileo. Tillich's "God above God" as the ground of creativity and courage can, in this new millennium, enable religion to be reconciled with science. Religion is a source of the "courage to create," which is essential for progress in scientific knowledge. Religion and science working together as complementary dimensions of the human spirit can lead us into a wider world and greater wisdom. Reconciliation and reunion characterize the New Being and Creation. 相似文献
7.
8.
Cheryl A. Burgess Irving Kirsch Howard Shane Kristen L. Niederauer Steven M. Graham & Alyson Bacon 《Psychological science》1998,9(1):71-74
Forty college students were taught facilitated communication via a commercially available training videotape. They were then asked to facilitate the communication of a confederate, who was described as developmentally disabled and unable to speak. All 40 participants produced responses that they attributed at least partially to the confederate, and most attributed all of the communication entirely to her. Eighty-nine percent produced responses corresponding to information they had received, most of which was unknown to the confederate. Responding was significantly correlated with simple ideomotor responses with a pendulum and was not affected by information about the controversy surrounding facilitated communication. These data support the hypothesis that facilitated communication is an instance of automatic writing, akin to that observed in hypothesis and with Ouija boards, and that the ability to produce automatic writing is more common than previously thought. 相似文献
9.
Word frequency is one of the strongest determiners of reaction time (RT) in word recognition tasks; it is an important theoretical and methodological variable. The Ku?era and Francis (1967) word frequency count (derived from the 1-million-word Brown corpus) is used by most investigators concerned with the issue of word frequency. Word frequency estimates from the Brown corpus were compared with those from a 131-million-word corpus (the HAL corpus; conversational text gathered from Usenet) in a standard word naming task with 32 subjects. RT was predicted equally well by both corpora for high-frequency words, but the larger corpus provided better predictors for low- and medium-frequency words. Furthermore, the larger corpus provides estimates for 97,261 lexical items; the smaller corpus, for 50,406 items. 相似文献
10.
We evaluated the initial effectiveness, maintenance, and transferability of the results of functional communication training as an intervention for the challenging behaviors exhibited by 3 students. Assessment indicated that escape from academic demands was involved in the maintenance of the challenging behaviors. Social attention was also implicated as controlling the behavior of 1 student. The intervention involved teaching alternative assistance-seeking and attention-getting phrases to the students in an effort to replace challenging behavior with these verbal equivalents. Multiple baseline data collected across the 3 students indicated that not only did the intervention substantially reduce challenging behavior but also that these results transferred across new tasks, environments, and teachers, and were generally maintained from 18 to 24 months following the introduction of functional communication training. These results are discussed in light of recent efforts to develop effective interventions for severe challenging behavior and to understand the processes underlying transfer and maintenance of intervention effects. 相似文献