首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3848篇
  免费   199篇
  2023年   27篇
  2022年   25篇
  2021年   53篇
  2020年   83篇
  2019年   87篇
  2018年   123篇
  2017年   142篇
  2016年   138篇
  2015年   116篇
  2014年   116篇
  2013年   464篇
  2012年   207篇
  2011年   188篇
  2010年   107篇
  2009年   127篇
  2008年   170篇
  2007年   147篇
  2006年   144篇
  2005年   107篇
  2004年   107篇
  2003年   85篇
  2002年   92篇
  2001年   60篇
  2000年   67篇
  1999年   63篇
  1998年   41篇
  1997年   31篇
  1996年   36篇
  1994年   23篇
  1993年   30篇
  1992年   51篇
  1991年   37篇
  1990年   40篇
  1989年   26篇
  1988年   41篇
  1987年   39篇
  1985年   31篇
  1984年   31篇
  1983年   28篇
  1980年   26篇
  1979年   45篇
  1977年   22篇
  1976年   31篇
  1975年   25篇
  1974年   27篇
  1973年   24篇
  1972年   25篇
  1971年   28篇
  1968年   28篇
  1967年   22篇
排序方式: 共有4047条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Human and experimental animal research suggests that social stress in general--and chronic, effortful attempts to exert social control in particular--may contribute to cardiovascular disease. We examined the effects of exerting social influence or control on cardiovascular responses in married couples. Compared to husbands discussing a problem with their wives, husbands attempting to influence or persuade their wives displayed larger increases in systolic blood pressure (SBP) before and during the discussion. Furthermore, these physiological effects were accompanied by increases in anger and a more hostile and coldly assertive interpersonal style. Although wives who engaged in social influence attempts displayed generally similar behavior, they did not show the elevated SBP response or anger. We discuss the results in terms of the social context of cardiovascular reactivity and potential marital factors in cardiovascular health.  相似文献   
2.
3.
4.
5.
How do speakers design what they say in order to communicate effectively with groups of addressees who vary in their background knowledge of the topic at hand? Prior findings indicate that when a speaker addresses a pair of listeners with discrepant knowledge, that speakers Aim Low, designing their utterances for the least knowledgeable of the two addressees. Here, we test the hypothesis that speakers will depart from an Aim Low approach in order to efficiently communicate with larger groups of interacting partners. Further, we ask whether the cognitive demands of tracking multiple conversational partners' perspectives places limitations on successful audience design. We find that speakers can successfully track information about what up to four of their partners do and do not know in conversation. When addressing groups of 3–4 addressees at once, speakers design language based on the combined knowledge of the group. These findings point to an audience design process that simultaneously represents the perspectives of multiple other individuals and combines these representations in order to design utterances that strike a balance between the different needs of the individuals within the group.  相似文献   
6.
7.
Do people represent space, time, number, and other conceptual domains using a generalized magnitude system (GMS)? To answer this question, numerous studies have used the spatial‐numerical association of response codes (SNARC) task and its variants. Yet, for a combination of reasons, SNARC‐like effects cannot provide evidence for a GMS, even in principle. Rather, these effects support a broader theory of how people use space metaphorically to scaffold their understanding of myriad non‐spatial domains, whether or not these domains exhibit variation in magnitude.  相似文献   
8.
9.
10.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号