全文获取类型
收费全文 | 125篇 |
免费 | 26篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
156篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 1篇 |
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 2篇 |
2018年 | 4篇 |
2017年 | 4篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 19篇 |
2012年 | 5篇 |
2011年 | 3篇 |
2010年 | 3篇 |
2009年 | 3篇 |
2008年 | 6篇 |
2007年 | 13篇 |
2006年 | 7篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 8篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 5篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有156条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
Laurence G. Weinzimmer Heidi M. Baumann Daniel P. Gullifor Veronika Koubova 《The Journal of social psychology》2017,157(3):322-337
In this study, we examine the dynamics between emotional intelligence, work-family balance, and job performance. A review of the literature to date has shown distinct relationships between emotional intelligence to job performance and work-family balance to job performance. We utilize a sample of 233 respondents to empirically test our set of hypotheses that contend work-family balance mediates the relationship between emotional intelligence and job performance. Our results support these hypotheses. Practical implications and directions for future research are discussed. 相似文献
22.
23.
Evangelia Demerouti Arnold B. Bakker Patricia Voydanoff 《European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology》2013,22(2):128-149
The study examines whether home life influences inrole and extrarole job performance in positive or negative ways. We hypothesized that home resources would have a direct positive effect on job performance, and an indirect relationship through home–work facilitation (HWF). Home demands were predicted to negatively affect job performance through home–work interference (HWI). These hypotheses were tested on a sample of 190 male employees. In addition to employees' self-reports, wives rated their own home demands and resources (these were used as controls), and co-workers rated the men's HWI, HWF, and job performance. Results partially supported the hypotheses. Men's home demands and resources were related to self-rated HWI and HWF respectively, whereas co-worker ratings of HWI and HWF were associated with inrole and extrarole performance respectively. Men's home resources also had a direct relationship with inrole and extrarole performance. There was no convincing evidence that HWI and HWF acted as mediators in these relationships. Taken together, the positive influence of home life on job performance was stronger than the negative one. 相似文献
24.
How does mere social presence affect cognitive processes? The extant literature has focused on the impact of social presence on cognitive resources. The present study extends this work by focusing on the positivity of cognitive appraisal. Building on recent findings it was predicted that the traits neuroticism and impression management will differentially moderate the effect, such that neuroticism will be associated with a negative shift in appraisal, and impression management with a positive shift. In an experiment, participants (N = 158) formed evaluations of life events either alone or in social presence. The results supported the predictions. The findings advance the knowledge about the effect of social presence on cognition, and about the role of personality in moderating responses in public social contexts. 相似文献
25.
26.
Imitation: definitions, evidence, and mechanisms 总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0
Zentall TR 《Animal cognition》2006,9(4):335-353
Imitation can be defined as the copying of behavior. To a biologist, interest in imitation is focused on its adaptive value for the survival of the organism, but to a psychologist, the mechanisms responsible for imitation are the most interesting. For psychologists, the most important cases of imitation are those that involve demonstrated behavior that the imitator cannot see when it performs the behavior (e.g., scratching one's head). Such examples of imitation are sometimes referred to as opaque imitation because they are difficult to account for without positing cognitive mechanisms, such as perspective taking, that most animals have not been acknowledged to have. The present review first identifies various forms of social influence and social learning that do not qualify as opaque imitation, including species-typical mechanisms (e.g., mimicry and contagion), motivational mechanisms (e.g., social facilitation, incentive motivation, transfer of fear), attentional mechanisms (e.g., local enhancement, stimulus enhancement), imprinting, following, observational conditioning, and learning how the environment works (affordance learning). It then presents evidence for different forms of opaque imitation in animals, and identifies characteristics of human imitation that have been proposed to distinguish it from animal imitation. Finally, it examines the role played in opaque imitation by demonstrator reinforcement and observer motivation. Although accounts of imitation have been proposed that vary in their level of analysis from neural to cognitive, at present no theory of imitation appears to be adequate to account for the varied results that have been found. 相似文献
27.
Charles H. Shea David L. Wright Gabriele Wulf Chad Whitacre 《Journal of motor behavior》2013,45(1):27-36
In 2 experiments, the authors studied the effectiveness of physical and observational practice on learning and the effect on learning of combining physical practice and observation, as compared with providing physical practice alone. In Experiment 1, retention and transfer performance of 30 university students after physical, observational, or no practice were contrasted. Consistent with findings from other studies, the retention results indicated that observational practice is inferior to physical practice. The transfer data indicated no differences between observation and physical practice groups. In Experiment 2, retention and transfer performance of 30 participants in physical and combined (alternating physical and observational) practice groups were contrasted. The retention results showed no differences between the combined and physical practice groups, but the combined group performed significantly better than the physical practice group on the transfer test. Those findings suggest that a combination of observation and physical practice permits unique opportunities for learning beyond those available via either practice regimen alone. 相似文献
28.
Maggie Gale 《Thinking & reasoning》2013,19(3):294-315
Wason's standard 2-4-6 task requires discovery of a single rule and leads to around 20% solutions, whereas the dual goal (DG) version requires discovery of two rules and elevates solutions to over 60%. We report an experiment that aimed to discriminate between competing accounts of DG facilitation by manipulating the degree of complementarity between the to-be-discovered rules. Results indicated that perfect rule complementarity is not essential for task success, thereby undermining a key tenet of the goal complementarity account of DG facilitation. The triple heterogeneity account received a good degree of support since more varied triple exploration was associated with facilitatory DG conditions, in line with this account's prediction that task success is associated with the creative search of the problem space. The contrast class account (an extension of Oaksford & Chater's, 1994, iterative counterfactual model) was also corroborated in that the generation of descending triples was demonstrated to be the dominant predictor of DG success. We focus our discussion on conceptual ideas relating to the way in which iterative counterfactual testing and contrast class identification may work together to provide a powerful basis for effective hypothesis testing. 相似文献
29.
李虎;李建国;曾磊;苏凌洁 《心理科学进展》2025,33(3):411-427
随着现代通信技术发展, 非工作时间工作连通行为(Work Connectivity Behavior After-hours, 简称 WCBA)日益普遍且影响复杂。本研究基于71篇文献进行元分析发现: (1) WCBA既会通过工作自主性对工作−家庭平衡和任务绩效产生正向影响, 又会通过心理脱离对工作−家庭平衡和任务绩效产生负向影响; (2) 相较于心理脱离的负向中介作用, 工作自主性在WCBA与任务绩效之间的正向中介作用更强; 相较于工作自主性的正向中介作用, 心理脱离在WCBA与工作−家庭平衡之间的负向中介作用更强。本研究厘清了WCBA在家庭领域和工作领域差异化影响的中间机制, 对工作−家庭边界领域的研究进行了更新与补充。 相似文献
30.