全文获取类型
收费全文 | 525篇 |
免费 | 29篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
557篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 2篇 |
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 17篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 25篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 56篇 |
2012年 | 20篇 |
2011年 | 23篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 36篇 |
2007年 | 46篇 |
2006年 | 36篇 |
2005年 | 35篇 |
2004年 | 16篇 |
2003年 | 16篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有557条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
391.
Repetitive thought has been focused upon as a transdiagnostic risk factor for depression, anxiety, and poor physical health. Among the forms of repetitive thought, rumination and worry are considered to play important roles in the onset and maintenance of insomnia. However, there have been few attempts to clarify the similarities, differences, and interaction between the functions of rumination and worry in sleep problems. Furthermore, no study has investigated the prospective relationships between these two forms of repetitive thought and sleep disturbance. In the present study, we examined the prospective associations between repetitive thought and subjective sleep quality, measured by a self-report questionnaire. A total of 208 undergraduates participated in a 2-wave longitudinal survey with an interval of 3 weeks between assessments. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses showed that baseline rumination predicted reduction in the follow-up assessment of subjective sleep quality, controlling for levels of depressive and anxious symptoms. This main effect of rumination was qualified by the levels of worry; for individuals with higher levels of worry, rumination was associated with greater reduction in subjective sleep quality. These results suggest that both rumination and worry have unique associations with sleep and that their interaction is especially important in sleep problems. 相似文献
392.
《Journal of Global Ethics》2013,9(2-3):179-191
Given the importance of being able to account for moral obligations towards future generations, especially in the light of the problem of global climate change, I argue that there are under-appreciated notions in African thought that are able to significantly contribute to the on-going discourse with respect to inter-generational moral obligations. I identify two related African notions, both springing from the prominent belief that ancestors who have died – but continue to have a presence – are entitled to respect, which upon secular refinement are promising in terms of grounding a claim that we do have moral obligations to future generations. These conceptions are that the environment is a communal resource, shared across generations, and that the present generation should express gratitude to its predecessors for preserving the environment on its behalf, by emulating its predecessors and preserving the environment for future generations. I argue that these two conceptions present plausible grounds for thinking that we have moral obligations to posterity, partly because they go some way towards overcoming some of the theoretical concerns generally associated with the notion of moral obligations towards unidentifiable, contingent future persons. 相似文献
393.
Michael L. Commons Eric A. Goodheart Linda M. Bresette Nancy F. Bauer Edwin W. Farrell Katherine G. McCarthy Dorothy L. Danaher Francis A. Richards Janet B. Ellis Anne M. O'Brien Joseph A. Rodriguez Dawn Schraeder 《Journal of Adult Development》1995,2(3):193-199
This reply discusses the measurement of formal, systematic, and metasystematic stages. Recent multidomain studies have shown disparate results and we suggest that these results could be attributed not to actual variations in adult development or the existence or non-existence of stage, but instead may reflect differences in methodology regarding task, domain, and scoring. Our study indicates that when tested in only one domain—balance beams and extensions of those tasks—participants who performed at the higher stage were also able to perform at the lower stages. These results also coincide with similar studies conducted. 相似文献
394.
Janet Landman Elizabeth A. Vandewater Abigail J. Stewart Janet E. Malley 《Journal of Adult Development》1995,2(2):87-97
Counterfactual thinking entails the process of imagining alternatives to reality—what might have been. The present study examines the frequency, content, and emotional and cognitive concomitants of counterfactual thinking about past missed opportunities in midlife women. At age 43, nearly two-thirds of the sample of educated adult women reported having missed certain opportunities at some time in their lives. Most of the counterfactual thoughts concerned missed opportunities for greater challenge in work. Emotional distress at age 33 did not predict later counterfactual thought. Instead, counterfactual thinking at age 43 was associated with concurrent emotional distress. However, acknowledging counterfactual thinking about the past was also associated with envisioning ways to change things for the better in the future. This suggests the possibility that the negative appraisal often entailed in counterfactual thinking may be associated with emotional distress in the short run but with motivational benefits in the long run, at least for middle-aged women. 相似文献
395.
Christoph Hoerl 《Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences》2008,7(4):485-500
It is sometimes claimed that non-human animals (and perhaps also young children) live their lives entirely in the present
and are cognitively ‘stuck in time’. Adult humans, by contrast, are said to be able to engage in ‘mental time travel’. One
possible way of making sense of this distinction is in terms of the idea that animals and young children cannot engage in
tensed thought, which might seem a preposterous idea in the light of certain findings in comparative and developmental psychology.
I try to make this idea less preposterous by looking into some of the cognitive requirements for tensed thought. In particular,
I suggest that tensed thought requires a specific form of causal understanding, which animals and young children may not possess.
相似文献
Christoph HoerlEmail: |
396.
从《太玄》看扬雄的人性论思想 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
一般讲扬雄的人性论,都以《法言》为依据,认为他是“善恶混”派的代表.并认为他的人性论缺乏内在超越的根据,比较接近于荀子。本文认为《太玄》中蕴涵丰富的人性论思想,研究扬雄的人性论,应以《太玄》为基础。扬雄的人性论本于其天道观,有很强的超越性,扬氏以五行为架构,提出了非常具体的超越途径,其思想接近于性善说. 相似文献
397.
贾思勰的《齐民要术》在农业思想中蕴含着挥之不去的忧患意识,具体表现为:劝君以农为本,重视救荒农作物的栽培,发展基于大农业观的多种经营等,以期实现他那“要在安民,富而教之”的宏愿。 相似文献
398.
《周易》田猎习武的军训观 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
《周易》的军事思想是丰富的,它多侧面地涉及了对古代军事斗争现象的理解和概括,其中也包括了军事斗争中的重要组成部分——田猎习武。在《周易》所取田猎之象中,真切地体现着《周易》的战争观念、军训观念。其中反对躁于用武的田猎观;田猎所体现的古朴的人道精神;田猎之义是为诛除丑类;以田猎形式来娴习征战技艺;田猎之获,演习着战争胜利的意义等等,质而言之,这就是它的战争观念。本文即专题探讨《周易》对古代田猎习武现象的认识及其所具有的观念。 相似文献
399.
本研究以81名篮球专业的大学生为被试,通过决策篮球视频,考察运动决策中的无意识思维效应。实验1探讨了不同运动决策信息下的无意识思维决策差异,发现呈现直觉性决策信息时,无意识思维组的决策效果好于意识思维组。实验2在无意识思维阶段通过启动被试的加工方式,考察直觉性决策信息下,启动不同的加工方式对运动决策的无意识思维效应的影响,发现整体加工组的无意识思维决策效果最佳。研究结果表明,直觉性决策信息下,启动被试的整体加工方式,其无意识思维的决策效果会有所提升。 相似文献
400.
Treating combat deployed soldiers is becoming more prevalent and needed in psychiatry. Modern combat produces unique psychological challenges, including those without criteria for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). This article will attempt to share the primary author's experience with psychotherapy in a combat zone, along with understanding the general themes of dreams the author encountered while being deployed. Toward that end, the primary author [RW] discusses his personal experiences in Iraq working with soldiers whom he saw and treated while in theatre, with a particular focus on the dreams they reported. The co-authors [EG and MI] afterward collaborated with the primary author to formulate and provide insight into the dreams from a Jungian perspective. 相似文献