首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   503篇
  免费   95篇
  国内免费   46篇
  2023年   12篇
  2022年   16篇
  2021年   19篇
  2020年   29篇
  2019年   27篇
  2018年   40篇
  2017年   35篇
  2016年   26篇
  2015年   31篇
  2014年   34篇
  2013年   92篇
  2012年   9篇
  2011年   16篇
  2010年   11篇
  2009年   21篇
  2008年   28篇
  2007年   24篇
  2006年   20篇
  2005年   19篇
  2004年   13篇
  2003年   18篇
  2002年   8篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   3篇
  1996年   8篇
  1995年   9篇
  1994年   3篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   6篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   7篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   1篇
排序方式: 共有644条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
571.
近些年, 发展可塑性领域出现两个新模型--差别易感性模型和优势敏感性模型, 两个模型均表明个体从积极环境中的获益具有差异性。而心理干预作为一种特殊的积极环境被引入到该领域的研究中。已有的研究已经发现一些证据表明, 具备某些特征的当事人从心理干预中获益更多。这些特征包括人格和气质类、遗传类和生理类。本质上, 这些研究在处理“什么对谁最有效”的问题, 但现有研究仅能部分回答这个问题。因此, 心理干预的效果差异有多少可归于这些易感特征, 是有待继续回答的问题。  相似文献   
572.
刘文  朱琳  张雪  张玉  刘颖 《心理学报》2015,47(11):1341-1348
公平敏感性是指个体对感知到的公平和不公平的稳定而个性化的反应, 其表现为个体对公平的不同偏好。本研究旨在考察2~3岁儿童公平敏感性发展特点, 并进一步探究分配情境、分配数量和分配结果等因素对儿童公平敏感性的影响。实验1采用VOE范式, 在第三方任务情境下, 考察130名2~3岁儿童公平敏感性的发展特点, 结果发现在VOE范式下, 2~3岁儿童公平敏感性随着年龄增长而上升, 3岁~3岁3个月儿童的公平敏感性发展最明显。实验2, 选取60名处于公平敏感性发展最明显的3岁~3岁3个月的儿童比较其在不同分配情境、分配数量以及分配结果下的注视时间差异。结果表明, 在有接受者情境下儿童公平敏感性显著高于无接受者情境下的公平敏感性, 并且公平敏感性受分配数量的影响。  相似文献   
573.
Ethics and Nanotechnology: Views of Nanotechnology Researchers   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
Robert McGinn 《Nanoethics》2008,2(2):101-131
A study was conducted of nanotechnology (NT) researchers’ views about ethics in relation to their work. By means of a purpose-built questionnaire, made available on the Internet, the study probed NT researchers’ general attitudes toward and beliefs about ethics in relation to NT, as well as their views about specific NT-related ethical issues. The questionnaire attracted 1,037 respondents from 13 U.S. university-based NT research facilities. Responses to key questionnaire items are summarized and noteworthy findings presented. For most respondents, the ethical responsibilities of NT researchers are not limited to those related to safety and integrity in the laboratory. Most believe that NT researchers also have specific ethical responsibilities to the society in which their research is done and likely to be applied. NT appears to be one of the first areas of contemporary technoscientific activity in which a long-standing belief is being seriously challenged: the belief that society is solely responsible for what happens when a researcher’s work, viewed as neutral and merely enabling, is applied in a particular social context. Survey data reveal that most respondents strongly disagree with that paradigmatic belief. Finally, an index gauging NT researcher sensitivity to ethics and ethical issues related to NT was constructed. A substantial majority of respondents exhibited medium or high levels of sensitivity to ethics in relation to NT. Although most respondents view themselves as not particularly well informed about ethics in relation to NT, a substantial majority are aware of and receptive to ethical issues related to their work, and believe that these issues merit consideration by society and study by current and future NT practitioners.
Robert McGinnEmail:
  相似文献   
574.
The purpose of this study was to examine the role of foot sole somatosensory information during reactive postural control. Twenty young adults (22.0 ± 1.4 y) participated in this study. Baseline skin sensitivity from the foot sole was assessed using Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments. Postural muscle responses, in the form of electromyographic (EMG) onset latencies and amplitudes, were then obtained while participants recovered their balance while standing on a moveable platform that could translate in either the forward or backward direction. Following these baseline measures, the participant’s foot soles were immersed in a 0–2 °C ice-water bath for 12 min followed by a 3 min re-immersion period. At the completion of foot cooling, foot sole sensitivity and postural muscle responses to the balance perturbations were re-assessed. Results indicated that the foot cooling protocol reduced foot sole sensitivity and remained reduced throughout the duration of the experiment (p < 0.001). The reduction in foot sole somatosensation resulted in the soleus EMG onset latency being delayed by 3 ms (p = 0.041) and the soleus and medial gastrocnemius EMG amplitudes increasing by 14–23% (p = 0.002–0.036) during the balance perturbation trials. While the magnitude of these results may suggest that foot cooling has a minor functional consequence on reactive postural control, it is likely that the results also reflect the ability of the central nervous system to rapidly adapt to situations with altered somatosensory feedback.  相似文献   
575.
Adolescents' temperamental frustration is a developmental precursor of adult neuroticism and psychopathology. Because the mechanisms that underlie the prospective association between adolescents' high frustration and psychopathology (internalizing/externalizing) have not been studied extensively, we quantified three pathways: stress generation [mediation via selection/evocation of stressful life events (SLEs)], cross‐sectional frustration‐psychopathology overlap (‘carry‐over’/common causes), and a direct (non‐mediated) vulnerability effect of frustration, including moderation of SLE impact. Frustration and psychopathology were assessed at age 16 with the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire and the Youth Self‐Report. No gender differences in frustration were observed. At age 19, psychopathology was reassessed by using the Adult Self‐Report, while occurrence of endogenous (self‐generated) and exogenous (not self‐generated) SLEs during the interval (ages 16–19) were ascertained with the Life Stress Interview, an investigator‐based contextual‐stressfulness rating procedure (N = 957). Half of the prospective effect of frustration on psychopathology was explained by baseline overlap, including effects of ‘carry‐over’ and common causes, about 5% reflected stress generation (a ‘vicious’ cycle with the environment adolescents navigate and shape), and 45% reflected unmediated association: a direct vulnerability effect including stress sensitivity or moderation of SLE impact. After adjustment for their overlap, frustration predicted the development of externalizing but not internalizing symptoms. Copyright © 2016 European Association of Personality Psychology  相似文献   
576.
577.
Most theories of visual search maintain that attention is selectively tuned to the attributes of the search target (e.g., orange). Conversely, according to the relational account, attention is biased to the relative feature of the target (e.g., redder). However, previous studies that supported the relational account mainly measured mean response times. Hence, the results might not reflect early, perceptual mechanisms (e.g., signal enhancement) but later, decision-based mechanisms (channel selection). The current study tested the relational account against feature-specific theories in a spatial cueing task, in which the targets were backward-masked, and target identification accuracy was measured. The first experiment corroborated earlier results, demonstrating that relational effects are due to signal enhancement. In the second experiment, we chose highly discriminable colours along the blue–red continuum, and obtained results that were more consistent with broad feature-specific rather than relational tuning. The implications of these findings for current theories of attention are discussed.  相似文献   
578.
Anxiety sensitivity is a known precursor to panic attacks and panic disorder, and involves the misinterpretation of anxiety-related sensations. Aerobic exercise has been shown to reduce generalized anxiety, and may also reduce anxiety sensitivity through exposure to feared physiological sensations. Accordingly, 54 participants with elevated anxiety sensitivity scores completed six 20-min treadmill exercise sessions at either a high-intensity aerobic ( n = 29 ) or low-intensity ( n = 25 ) level. Self-ratings of anxiety sensitivity, fear of physiological sensations associated with anxiety, and generalized anxiety were obtained at pre-treatment, post-treatment, and one-week follow-up. Results indicated that both high- and low-intensity exercise reduced anxiety sensitivity. However, high-intensity exercise caused more rapid reductions in a global measure of anxiety sensitivity and produced more treatment responders than low-intensity exercise. Only high-intensity exercise reduced fear of anxiety-related bodily sensations. The implications of these findings are discussed.  相似文献   
579.
Anxiety sensitivity (AS) has been proposed as a risk factor for the development of panic disorder. Strong familial-genetic influences in panic disorder (PD) have been reported. AS may be familial too. The current study therefore examined whether first-degree relatives of PD patients are more anxiety-sensitive than normals. Twenty-three first-degree relatives of PD patients, 38 PD patients and 30 normals were given the Anxiety Sensitive Index and the Body Sensations Questionnaire. It was found that the first-degree relatives were, indeed, more anxiety-sensitive than the normals, but less so than the PD patients, suggesting that AS runs in families.  相似文献   
580.
Peritraumatic and persistent panic attacks in acute stress disorder   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
This study examined the prevalence of peritraumatic and persistent panic symptoms following trauma. Survivors of civilian trauma (n=30) with either acute stress disorder (ASD) or no acute stress disorder (non-ASD) were administered the Panic Module of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID). Participants also completed the Impact of Event Scale, Acute Stress Disorder Scale, Beck Depression Inventory, Beck Anxiety Inventory, and the Anxiety Sensitivity Index. Panic attacks were experienced by 77% of participants during their trauma, and 47% reported recurrent panic attacks post-trauma. ASD participants demonstrated more panic symptoms during and after their trauma than non-ASD participants. Posttraumatic panic was most strongly associated with anxiety sensitivity. These findings are discussed in terms of cognitive factors that may mediate posttrauma panic and treatment implications for managing posttraumatic anxiety.There is increasing evidence that panic attacks play a role in psychopathological response to trauma. A significant proportion of people with panic disorder report a history of trauma (). Moreover, two-thirds of trauma survivors report panic attacks within the previous 2 weeks (). There is also evidence that people with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) display elevated levels of anxiety sensitivity (). Recent attention has focused on acute panic reactions because of proposals that panic during trauma may condition trauma-related cues to subsequent panic (). There is evidence that panic attacks occur in 53-90% of trauma survivors during the traumatic experience (). Further, people with acute stress disorder (ASD) are more likely to report peritraumatic panic attacks than non-ASD individuals. ASD is a useful framework in which to investigate the role of panic in posttraumatic stress because ASD describes acute responses to trauma that are strongly predictive of chronic PTSD ().This study investigated the relationship between peritraumatic panic and ongoing panic attacks following trauma. Specifically, we indexed panic attacks during trauma and subsequent to trauma in trauma survivors with and without ASD. We also indexed the extent to which distorted interpretations about somatic sensations may be associated with panic attacks following trauma. We considered that the strong evidence that maladaptive appraisals of somatic sensations mediate panic () is directly relevant to posttraumatic panic. We hypothesized that ASD participants would report more peritraumatic and persistent panic than non-ASD participants, and that this panic would be associated with dysfunctional interpretations about somatic stimuli.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号