全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2534篇 |
免费 | 243篇 |
国内免费 | 79篇 |
专业分类
2856篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 9篇 |
2024年 | 43篇 |
2023年 | 70篇 |
2022年 | 64篇 |
2021年 | 76篇 |
2020年 | 149篇 |
2019年 | 161篇 |
2018年 | 177篇 |
2017年 | 181篇 |
2016年 | 141篇 |
2015年 | 122篇 |
2014年 | 110篇 |
2013年 | 319篇 |
2012年 | 90篇 |
2011年 | 82篇 |
2010年 | 59篇 |
2009年 | 69篇 |
2008年 | 101篇 |
2007年 | 133篇 |
2006年 | 99篇 |
2005年 | 94篇 |
2004年 | 58篇 |
2003年 | 49篇 |
2002年 | 55篇 |
2001年 | 30篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 22篇 |
1998年 | 22篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有2856条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
Florence W. Kaslow PhD Kjell Hansson PhD Anne-Marie Lundblad MA 《Contemporary Family Therapy》1994,16(6):521-537
This international, five-country long term marriages study of couples who have been married, or unmarried and cohabiting, for 20 years or longer is designed to study, among other things, marital satisfaction. This first report of the Swedish part of the study reports on a Swedish version of the self-rating Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS) used to investigate self-rated marital satisfaction. The couples describe high marital satisfaction and a high sense of coherence (SOC) compared to other groups. Contrary to other studies, no differences were found between men and women regarding SOC. The differences between them, that describe high relative to low marital satisfaction, pertain to matters of recreation, sex, life philosophy, time spent together, and frequency of quarrels. The men reported greater marital satisfaction than the women. 相似文献
12.
A new technique for determining children's subjective organization of speech is described. The technique consists of teaching a child a sentence and then repeatedly (1) reading the sentence up to a selected point, (2) sounding a tone, and (3) having the child supply the next word. The time from the tone to the onset of the child's voice is the dependent variable. In the first study 6- and 8-year-old children memorized the following sentence: “John likes to feed the little puppies in the barn.” Their response times suggested that the dominant segmentation pattern consisted of four phrases: a subject phrase, a verb phrase, an object phrase and a modifier phrase. Within each phrase differences between reaction times were on the order of 30 msec. Across phrases they were on the order of 120 msec. The same trend emerged for subsamples of the children with various short-term memory sizes, and for adults. It also appeared for subsamples of the total scores (e.g., first six trials and last six trials). In order to provide further evidence of the technique's validity, different sentences of the same general type were presented to children with short-term memory spans of either 3, 4, or 5. As predicted, it was found that when children's spans corresponded to the number of hypothesized chunks (i.e., 4), they could repeat the sentences verbatim on the first exposure. They could not do so, however, when their spans were lower than that value or when the number of phrases was increased. It was concluded that the new technique is a promising one for assessing young children's patterns of speech segmentation. 相似文献
13.
Joseph A. Corcoran 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》1980,5(2):99-114
This study investigated the effects of presumably positive and neutral response-contingent stimuli on the occurence of stuttering. Eight adult stutterers spoke spontaneously during a 60-min experimental session, which was divided into three 20-min periods. During Condition I no stimuli were delivered. During Condition II a digital counter and a pair of yellow lights were activated each time the subject stuttered. During Condition III these same response-contingent stimuli were delivered, but with the additional factor that the subject would earn 5¢ each time the counter moved forward and the lights were illuminated. Conditions II and III were counterbalanced across subjects. The experimenter was unaware of which of the two contingencies were operative during Conditions II and III. Results showed no significant changes in occurence of stuttering due to either response-contingent stimulus. A discussion of theoretical and experimental implications is given. 相似文献
14.
Nan E. Bernstein 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》1981,6(4):341-350
Analysis of the spontaneous speech of eight stuttering and eight normally fluent children reveals that (a) the location of both stuttering and normal disfluent moments is constrained by sentential constituent structure, and (b) stuttering children experience fluency breakdown with a particular type of constituent (verb phrase) not usually disfluently produced by normals. A view of early stuttering as a sentence planning and integration disorder is present. 相似文献
15.
In recent years, several studies have been conducted in which the fluency of stutterers was compared with that of normal speakers. These investigations have been of two basic types, some involving objective measures of physiologic, aerodynamic, or acoustic features of the subjects' fluent speech signal, and others involving experiments performed in which listeners attempted to distinguish the fluency produced by stutterers from that generated by normal speakers. The body of literature pertaining to each of these two types of investigations is reviewed and interpreted. The interpretations offered are then developed in an effort to shed further light on both fluency and stuttering. Implications for further research, as well as for the evaluation, treatment, and post-therapy assessment of stutterers are drawn. 相似文献
16.
Robert L. Hale 《Journal of School Psychology》1981,19(3):274-278
This study investigated the efficacy of predicting academic achievement as measured by the Wide Range Achievement Test using the factor scores of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised as potential predictors. Both instruments were administered to 200 school children referred for psychological evaluations. Factor scores for the WISC-R were computed according to equations provided by Gutkin (1978). These factor scores then were regressed in a stepwise manner on the Reading, Spelling, and Arithmetic standard scores from the Wide Range Achievement Test. Stepwise regressions also were performed using the traditional Verbal and Performance IQs as the potential predictors. A comparison was made between the results obtained by use of the factors or the more common scores. The results indicated that the Freedom From Distractibility factor score significantly aided in the prediction of Reading and Spelling achievement and was of primary importance in predicting Arithmetic achievement. When the stepwise results using the different scores were contrasted, the factor scores accounted for appreaciably more variance in Arithmetic than did the traditional Verbal and Performance IQs. Derived regression equations are reported along with a discussion of the interpretability of the Freedom From Distractibility factor. 相似文献
17.
Patrick Suppes Michael Cohen Robert Laddaga James Anliker Robert Floyd 《Journal of mathematical psychology》1983,27(4):341-369
A procedural theory of eye movement that accounts for main features of the stochastic behavior of eye-fixation durations and direction of movement of saccades in the process of solving arithmetic exercises of addition and subtraction is presented. The best-fitting distribution of fixation durations with a relatively simple theoretical justification consists of a mixture of an exponential distribution and the convolution of two exponential distributions. The eye movements themselves were found to approximate a random walk that fits rather closely in both adult and juvenile subjects the motion postulated by the normative algorithm ordinarily taught in schools. Certain structural features of addition and subtraction exercises, such as the number of columns, the presence or absence of a carry or a borrow, are well known to affect their difficulty. In this study, regressions on such structural variables were found to account for only a relatively small part of the variation in eye-fixation durations. 相似文献
18.
19.
Volodar V. Kraevskii 《Studies in Philosophy and Education》1991,11(1):45-49
An analysis is provided detailing the conditions for the appropriate theoretical study of the object of educational science: pedagogy. 相似文献
20.
Simultaneous observational recordings were made in vivo, via an observation mirror, and via closed circuit television. Three of nine observers had extensive experience recording behavior in vivo; three had extensive experience recording behavior via mirror; and three had extensive experience recording via television. Observers recorded nine categories of disruptive behavior for children in a special class setting. Frequencies of behavior recorded in vivo, via mirror, and via television differed significantly for only one category, vocalization. There were no significant main effects or interactions involving the observers' previous experience. Occurrence reliability coefficients computed within and between media demonstrated the similarity of observer agreement in all three media. Data collection procedures using an observational mirror or closed circuit television appear to be reasonable alternatives to in vivo observation in circumstances similar to those in the present study. 相似文献