全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6660篇 |
免费 | 2374篇 |
国内免费 | 362篇 |
专业分类
9396篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 58篇 |
2022年 | 59篇 |
2021年 | 178篇 |
2020年 | 247篇 |
2019年 | 667篇 |
2018年 | 553篇 |
2017年 | 678篇 |
2016年 | 621篇 |
2015年 | 536篇 |
2014年 | 507篇 |
2013年 | 749篇 |
2012年 | 355篇 |
2011年 | 356篇 |
2010年 | 313篇 |
2009年 | 269篇 |
2008年 | 269篇 |
2007年 | 364篇 |
2006年 | 365篇 |
2005年 | 296篇 |
2004年 | 282篇 |
2003年 | 230篇 |
2002年 | 192篇 |
2001年 | 143篇 |
2000年 | 116篇 |
1999年 | 70篇 |
1998年 | 29篇 |
1997年 | 29篇 |
1996年 | 17篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 62篇 |
1984年 | 69篇 |
1983年 | 45篇 |
1982年 | 49篇 |
1981年 | 52篇 |
1980年 | 52篇 |
1979年 | 48篇 |
1978年 | 81篇 |
1977年 | 66篇 |
1976年 | 72篇 |
1975年 | 59篇 |
1974年 | 53篇 |
1973年 | 49篇 |
排序方式: 共有9396条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
82.
83.
84.
85.
86.
M Beth Casey 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1976,22(3):468-477
Methods of increasing learning efficiency through a blocking technique were examined in young children. An experimental group of children was trained on an initial task with one relevant dimension, followed by redundancy training in which an incidental dimension also became relevant. A control group given only redundancy training was compared to this group to test how much the experimental subjects blocked or screened out the incidental dimension. In order to increase blocking, three methods were used: a preset transfer problem, an overtraining on the initial task, and a delay following training. The most important finding was increased blocking through the Preset Condition. Overtraining and Delay Conditions had no effect. In addition, more slow learners on the initial task learned about the incidental dimension than fast or intermediate learners. Also, fewer subjects given form on the initial task learned about the incidental dimension than those given color. The Combined Blocking, Delay, and Overtraining group showed partial blocking, while the Preset group demonstrated complete blocking. No developmental differences occurred. The results were interpreted using a modification of Trabasso and Bower's attention model (Attention in learning: Theory and research. New York: Wiley, 1968). 相似文献
87.
Ben A Williams 《Learning and motivation》1976,7(3):418-430
The correlation between short-term retention of the outcome of the preceding response and overall learning proficiency was investigated for serial reversal learning. Pigeons were trained to asymptote on a serial reversal problem and then were presented a percentage reinforcement schedule where only some correct trials were rewarded. Nonrewarded correct trials were treated exactly as incorrect trials. The difference in error probability following the two types of correct trials was then used as a measure of short-term retention. When intertrial intervals (ITI) were short (6 sec), substantial differences occurred. When the ITI's were increased, the difference in accuracy declined regularly to no difference at an ITI of 60 sec. This demonstration of a short-term retention gradient, coupled with the finding that overall reversal learning was much better with the shorter ITI's, suggests that a primary mechanism of improvement in serial reversal learning is the acquisition of a conditional discrimination based on the outcome of the preceding response. 相似文献
88.
Stanley J. Weiss 《Learning and motivation》1976,7(4):477-516
The associations formed in the components of a multiple schedule can be classified as (1) stimulus-response (S-R) associations and (2) stimulus-reinforcer (S-SR) associations. The present experiments sought to determine the individual contribution of these S-R and S-SR associations to stimulus control by manipulating them independently. Responses postponed shocks by 25 sec in the presence of a tone alone and a light alone in all experiments. The contingencies programmed in the absence of both tone and light established a reference for the S-R and S-SR associations in tone and in light. All four possible combinations of signalling response increase or decrease together with incentive increase or decrease were studied. Although the influence of the contingencies operating in the absence of tone and light was difficult to detect from response rates in tone or light, presenting tone and light together revealed clear effects. Response rates in tone and light together relative to those in either alone depended upon the contingency operating in the absence of tone and light. Stimulus-response and stimulus-reinforcer associations appeared to counteract each other when in opposition and combine together to enhance each other when in agreement. This suggested that the associations of a stimulus to response and to incentive combine algebraically in determining stimulus control. An algebraic analysis in terms of the S-R and S-SR associations conditioned to the stimulus elements comprising the training and test stimuli accounted for the observed patterns of data. 相似文献
89.
This study systematically investigated the effects of different procedures on classroom behaviors ranging from teacher designated seating groups and seating groups based on sociometric data to peer encouragement and free time reinforcers. Subjects were first- and third-grade children in classrooms using a Follow Through program model. Data were taken on the attending behavior of the children in small groups arranged by the teacher. The level of appropriate attending behavior was low and did not increase when the groups were restructured based on sociometric data. The introduction of instructions to the peer groups to help the child with his or her work brought about a major increase in the third-grade subjects' appropriate behavior. Appropriate attending behavior for the first-grade children did not increase to the predetermined acceptable level until a free time period for each peer group was made contingent upon the appropriate behavior of the subject in each group. Frequencies of verbal interactions suggested peer attention has an effect on the appropriateness of peer interactions. 相似文献
90.
Draws on the experience of an innovative school mental health project, emphasizing early detection and prevention of school adjustment problems, to describe emergent, needed roles for school psychologists. The later include: systematic screening of primary graders and parent interviewing to further early detection; consultation with teachers and other school personnel; and recruitment, training, and supervision of nonprofessional help-agents. Such roles bring early effective services to many children, in ways that optimize their educational and personal development. 相似文献