全文获取类型
收费全文 | 23700篇 |
免费 | 2212篇 |
国内免费 | 526篇 |
专业分类
26438篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 246篇 |
2023年 | 407篇 |
2022年 | 400篇 |
2021年 | 476篇 |
2020年 | 883篇 |
2019年 | 880篇 |
2018年 | 831篇 |
2017年 | 942篇 |
2016年 | 911篇 |
2015年 | 626篇 |
2014年 | 808篇 |
2013年 | 2504篇 |
2012年 | 651篇 |
2011年 | 674篇 |
2010年 | 547篇 |
2009年 | 789篇 |
2008年 | 966篇 |
2007年 | 1060篇 |
2006年 | 1009篇 |
2005年 | 880篇 |
2004年 | 738篇 |
2003年 | 665篇 |
2002年 | 566篇 |
2001年 | 366篇 |
2000年 | 353篇 |
1999年 | 302篇 |
1998年 | 246篇 |
1997年 | 228篇 |
1996年 | 182篇 |
1995年 | 163篇 |
1994年 | 175篇 |
1993年 | 133篇 |
1992年 | 106篇 |
1991年 | 89篇 |
1990年 | 78篇 |
1988年 | 65篇 |
1987年 | 62篇 |
1985年 | 425篇 |
1984年 | 477篇 |
1983年 | 384篇 |
1982年 | 486篇 |
1981年 | 479篇 |
1980年 | 475篇 |
1979年 | 424篇 |
1978年 | 486篇 |
1977年 | 414篇 |
1976年 | 408篇 |
1975年 | 309篇 |
1974年 | 282篇 |
1973年 | 244篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
271.
272.
Current research has suggested that musical stimuli are processed in the right hemisphere except in musicians, in whom there is an increased involvement of the left hemisphere. The present study hypothesized that the more musical training persons receive, the more they will rely on an analytic/left-hemispheric processing strategy. The subjects were 10 faculty and 10 student musicians, and 10 faculty and 10 student nonmusicians. All subjects listened to a series of melodies (some recurring and some not) and excerpts (some actual and some not) in one ear and, after a rest, to a different series of melodies in the other ear. The task was to identify recurring vs. nonrecurring melodies and actual vs. nonactual excerpts. For student musicians, there was a right-ear/left-hemispheric advantage for melody recognition, while for student nonmusicians, the situation was the reverse. Neither faculty group showed any ear preference. There were no significant differences for excerpt recognition. Two possible explanations of the faculty performance were discussed in terms of physical maturation and a functionally more integrated hemispheric approach to the task. 相似文献
273.
The purpose of the present study was to determine if aphasic subject groups differentiated by the fluency of their verbal output employed rehearsal as a strategy for maintaining verbally coded information in primary memory. A task based upon the Brown-Peterson paradigm was administered to 10 fluent aphasic patients, 10 nonfluent aphasic patients, 10 right-brain-damaged patients, and 10 nonneurological patients. The findings indicate that the nonfluent aphasic patients did not rehearse the verbal information while the fluent aphasic, right-brain-damaged, and nonneurological patients did rehearse. In addition, both fluent and nonfluent aphasic patients encoded significantly less information into the memory system initially and performed worse on the task overall than right-brain-damaged and nonneurological patients. 相似文献
274.
275.
Mary Rees Nishio 《Brain and language》1981,13(2):259-289
A
, severely language-disabled boy was taught 250 words made up of one, two, three, or four Japanese/Chinese kanji. Many kanji words were learned and some ability to process these in simple sentences was observed. Acquisition of a word for meaning was not dependent on its visual complexity, but on the child's ability to grasp the meaning of the word. Results suggest that a whole-word method may facilitate very early reading acquisition and a possible neurophysiological correlate of this is proposed. Kanji or whole words may prove useful for language-handicapped individuals unable to process words phonetically. 相似文献
276.
This paper is a reply to an article in this journal by M. Zimmerman titled “The Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment: A Comment on Elardo and Bradley's Review”. We found Zimmerman's critique of our original review article (Developmental Review, 1981, 1, 113–145) to contain points which were well taken, and several which seemed pedantic. Specifically, we address his concerns about our discussion of the HOME scale's interrater reliability, test-retest stability, concurrent validity, and predictive validity; and we reply to his comments about the uses of the HOME scale for purposes of screening and matching environments. 相似文献
277.
The Social Readjustment Rating Scale (SRRS) was administered to 185 subjects whose age ranged from 21 to 61 yr. The responses were factor analyzed for a principal axis solution followed by isopromax oblique rotation. All the items were loaded and clustered under five factors labeled as follows: (1) personal growth, maturation, and renewal; (2) tension and uncertainty; transitions in personal or occupational situations; (3) changes in one's usual routine and relationship; (4) significant changes in family or mirriage; and (5) personal catastrophies. Males scored significantly higher than females on factors 4 and 5. There were no age differences in the obtained scores. The finding of the factorial structure was thought to add credence to the scale especially in view of past criticism. Implications for future research were also discussed. 相似文献
278.
A sample of 77 subjects responded to a questionnaire designed to collect information about their perceptions of the role of school psychologists. The results indicate that overall the respondents had a fairly accurate sense of what a school psychologist does, although they overestimated the importance of psychotherapy as one of the school psychologist's functions. It is recommended that school psychologists more clearly specify their function when dealing with members of the general public. 相似文献
279.
Robert L. Hale 《Journal of School Psychology》1981,19(3):274-278
This study investigated the efficacy of predicting academic achievement as measured by the Wide Range Achievement Test using the factor scores of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised as potential predictors. Both instruments were administered to 200 school children referred for psychological evaluations. Factor scores for the WISC-R were computed according to equations provided by Gutkin (1978). These factor scores then were regressed in a stepwise manner on the Reading, Spelling, and Arithmetic standard scores from the Wide Range Achievement Test. Stepwise regressions also were performed using the traditional Verbal and Performance IQs as the potential predictors. A comparison was made between the results obtained by use of the factors or the more common scores. The results indicated that the Freedom From Distractibility factor score significantly aided in the prediction of Reading and Spelling achievement and was of primary importance in predicting Arithmetic achievement. When the stepwise results using the different scores were contrasted, the factor scores accounted for appreaciably more variance in Arithmetic than did the traditional Verbal and Performance IQs. Derived regression equations are reported along with a discussion of the interpretability of the Freedom From Distractibility factor. 相似文献
280.
The fallacy of “two years below grade level for age” as a diagnostic criterion for reading disorders
The use of grade equivalent scores contrasted against grade placement is widespread in the diagnosis of dyslexia and other reading disabilities. This method substantially overstates disabilities at upper grade levels while underestimating the severity of difficulties in the early grades. Other difficulties and difficulties and distortions of this methods are also pointed out. An accurate, alternative method for reliably determining aptitude/achievement discrepancies is presented and its use discussed. 相似文献