全文获取类型
收费全文 | 20779篇 |
免费 | 1736篇 |
国内免费 | 1292篇 |
专业分类
23807篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 45篇 |
2023年 | 384篇 |
2022年 | 296篇 |
2021年 | 389篇 |
2020年 | 747篇 |
2019年 | 812篇 |
2018年 | 722篇 |
2017年 | 864篇 |
2016年 | 830篇 |
2015年 | 578篇 |
2014年 | 750篇 |
2013年 | 2318篇 |
2012年 | 475篇 |
2011年 | 629篇 |
2010年 | 529篇 |
2009年 | 757篇 |
2008年 | 951篇 |
2007年 | 1004篇 |
2006年 | 962篇 |
2005年 | 819篇 |
2004年 | 717篇 |
2003年 | 605篇 |
2002年 | 551篇 |
2001年 | 341篇 |
2000年 | 316篇 |
1999年 | 254篇 |
1998年 | 214篇 |
1997年 | 169篇 |
1996年 | 153篇 |
1995年 | 139篇 |
1994年 | 121篇 |
1993年 | 94篇 |
1992年 | 77篇 |
1991年 | 53篇 |
1990年 | 52篇 |
1989年 | 34篇 |
1988年 | 41篇 |
1985年 | 398篇 |
1984年 | 447篇 |
1983年 | 357篇 |
1982年 | 455篇 |
1981年 | 448篇 |
1980年 | 451篇 |
1979年 | 396篇 |
1978年 | 460篇 |
1977年 | 367篇 |
1976年 | 373篇 |
1975年 | 287篇 |
1974年 | 282篇 |
1973年 | 244篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
43.
Three samples of different levels of mental ability, but each of average or above- average intelligence, were administered a battery of reaction time test which measure the speed with which persons can execute various elementary cognitive processes. Discriminant analyses revealed significant differences between the groups in reaction times and intraindividual standard deviations, and univariate analyses indicated that higher ability groups tended to obtain significantly faster mean reaction times and to show significantly less intraindividual variability than lower ability groups. Groups which differed the most in mental ability differed to the greatest extent on the more complex reaction time tests but this pattern was reversed for groups which were relatively close in mean IQ. Generally, the results support the hypothesis that speed- of-processing is an important factor unferlying intelligence, but a number of puzzling findings indicate the need for continued research in this area. 相似文献
44.
Ann R. Eisenberg 《Argumentation》1987,1(2):113-125
The infant's first natural response when faced with opposition or when he opposes others' actions is to cry. As this kind of behavior becomes ineffective, the responses of the individuals with which he interacts force him to adopt more conventional — especially verbal — patterns of arguing, leading him to rational argumentation. The purpose of the present paper is to observe progressions in children's earliest verbal arguments and to see how and when they learn to adjust their strategies for different kinds of opponents (peers; parents; other adults). In order to examine the emergence of such persuasive strategies, and their distribution according to the different categories of opponents, systematic audiotape recordings of two Spanish-speaking girls between 2 and 3 years were analyzed. The data suggest that, although there were some differences in how the girls argued with parents versus peers, they were only beginning to adjust their speech to make it appropriate for one or the other type of listener. In general, they resorted to one dominant strategy for all listeners (insist, repeat, cry, scream, ...). Each girl, however, developed a small number of less frequently used strategies that she reserved for a subcategory of opponents. For example, one of them, Nancy, only threatened and insulted peers, while the other, Marisa, only used please and a temporizing strategy with parents. The girls used less adaptive, more agressive strategies (e.g. threats and insults) with their peers. With one exception, all of the girls' moves were self-centered. In fact, the girls had not yet reached the stage of rational argumentation. 相似文献
45.
Theories of observing differ in predicting whether or not a signal for absence of reinforcement (S−) is capable of reinforcing observing responses. Experiments in which S− was first removed from and then restored to the procedure have yielded mixed results. The present experiments suggest that failure to control for the direct effect of presenting S− may have been responsible. Pigeons and operant procedures were used. Experiment 1 showed that presentations of S−, even when not contingent on observing, can raise the rate of an observing response that was reinforced only by presentations of a signal (S+) that accompanied a schedule of food delivery. Experiment 2 showed that this effect resulted from bursts of responding that followed offsets of S−. Experiment 3 showed that, when the presence of S− was held constant, lower rates occurred when S− was dependent on, rather than independent of, observing. These results support theories that characterize S− as incapable of reinforcing observing responses. 相似文献
46.
It is suggested that molar streams of behavior are constructed of various arrangements of three elementary constituents (elicited, evoked, and emitted response classes). An eight-cell taxonomy is elaborated as a framework for analyzing and synthesizing complex behavioral repertoires based on these functional units. It is proposed that the local force binding functional units into a smoothly articulated kinetic sequence arises from temporally arranged relative response probability relationships. Behavioral integration is thought to reflect the joint influence of the organism's hierarchy of relative response probabilities, fluctuating biological states, and the arrangement of environmental and behavioral events in time. 相似文献
47.
Roger J. Bowden 《Psychometrika》1986,51(2):313-325
It is commonly held that even where questionnaire response is poor, correlational studies are affected only by loss of degrees of freedom or precision. We show that this supposition is not true. If the decision to respond is correlated with a substantive variable of interest, then regression or analysis of variance methods based upon the questionnaire results may be adversely affected by self-selection bias. Moreover such bias may arise even where response is 100%. The problem in both cases arises where selection information is passed to the score indirectly via the disturbance or individual effects, rather than entirely via the observable explanatory variables. We suggest tests for the ensuing self-selection bias and possible ways of handling the ensuing problems of inference.The author would like to thank members of the Psychology Department at UWA for references on the illustrative example, namely the effects of sex-stereotyping on achievements in mathematics; thanks are also due to the anonymous reviewers whose comments resulted in material improvements to the paper. 相似文献
48.
Patricia B. Sutker Daniel K. Winstead Kenneth C. Goist Robert M. Malow Albert N. Allain Jr. 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1986,8(2):89-101
Psychopathology and symptom patterns were studied in 60 former prisoners-of-war (POWs) by administering standardized tests including the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), an adjustment problem checklist, and a structured clinical interview. Most POWs showed marked psychological impairment, but modal profile analysis identified two prototypic MMPI patterns, which differed in pervasiveness and type of psychopathology. Profile subtypes were defined by unique clusters of clinical symptoms and differed in confinement stress severity. The typology of symptoms argues against a homogeneous conceptualization of stress-induced disorders and suggests the need for definition of the severity and subtype of stress phenomena and individual difference factors in responding to trauma. 相似文献
49.
Ronaldo Iachan 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1984,24(2):133-141
A measure of agreement for partially ordered data is suggested that makes use of the k categories with the highest ranks, the ranks of the remaining categories being disregarded. The proposed measure is applied to the Self-Directed Search in order to evaluate its agreement with occupational preference. Other forms of agreement in the Holland classification system can also be assessed by the proposed measure. Examples are congruence and husband-wife and counselor-client agreement. 相似文献
50.
Joan Daniels Pedro 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1984,25(1):80-95
Some form of work experience or internship ranks high on the list of criteria used by recruiters of college graduates for positions as management trainees. While internships are discussed positively by students, faculty, and recruiters, their evaluation has been faulted as being highly subjective and lacking in the use of controls and psychometric sophistication. This study used an Institutional Cycle design to investigate changes occurring after participation in internships with three cohorts of college age students. Changes were noted in both terminal and instrumental values and in work-specific needs. In the former, changes moved the profiles of the students closer to those of campus recruiters. In the latter, changes moved the students' needs in the direction of characteristics of the job, away from environmental characteristics. 相似文献