全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5355篇 |
免费 | 520篇 |
国内免费 | 132篇 |
专业分类
6007篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 79篇 |
2023年 | 117篇 |
2022年 | 84篇 |
2021年 | 85篇 |
2020年 | 190篇 |
2019年 | 230篇 |
2018年 | 183篇 |
2017年 | 219篇 |
2016年 | 177篇 |
2015年 | 134篇 |
2014年 | 196篇 |
2013年 | 652篇 |
2012年 | 134篇 |
2011年 | 160篇 |
2010年 | 170篇 |
2009年 | 162篇 |
2008年 | 220篇 |
2007年 | 281篇 |
2006年 | 323篇 |
2005年 | 256篇 |
2004年 | 262篇 |
2003年 | 213篇 |
2002年 | 169篇 |
2001年 | 110篇 |
2000年 | 91篇 |
1999年 | 69篇 |
1998年 | 51篇 |
1997年 | 52篇 |
1996年 | 29篇 |
1995年 | 35篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 25篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 65篇 |
1984年 | 69篇 |
1983年 | 44篇 |
1982年 | 52篇 |
1981年 | 54篇 |
1980年 | 51篇 |
1979年 | 49篇 |
1978年 | 82篇 |
1977年 | 65篇 |
1976年 | 71篇 |
1975年 | 58篇 |
1974年 | 53篇 |
1973年 | 49篇 |
排序方式: 共有6007条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
K Bloom 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1979,27(1):60-70
The use of the operant conditioning paradigm, as it has been applied to infant social, vocal behavior, reflects a failure to take into account the social nature of human infants over and above the rigid theoretical rationale of the paradigm. It is argued that: (1) the baseline procedure used in operant conditioning studies is methodologically and conceptually invalid; (2) the reinforcing stimulus used in social conditioning studies elicits the very response that it is assigned to reinforce; and (3) the effect of the response-reinforcer relationship is not the reinforcement (strengthening) of vocal response rate. 相似文献
52.
Susan T. Fiske Shelley E. Taylor Nancy L. Etcoff Jessica K. Laufer 《Journal of experimental social psychology》1979,15(4):356-377
Three studies were conducted to test whether imagery accounts for the effects of empathy on attributions, that is, whether attributers construct and scan a mental image of social scenarios the same way that actual participants scan a real environment. According to this interpretation, attributers “see” the world as the participant does and make vicarious attributions. An alternative interpretation holds that subjects base vicarious attributions on recreating the motivations and affect of the actor, so that imaging is irrelevant to empathy and attributions. In the first study, subjects who imagined a story from the perspective of a particular character later showed differential recall of story details as a function of role, but not differential attributions. In Experiment 2, role-taking subjects showed clear effects of imaged perspective on recall for story details, but no effects on attributions of causality for an accident. Further, recall and attribution were uncorrelated. In the last study, empathy and imagery role-taking instructions produced independent effects: imagers showed pronounced perspective-relevant recall and empathizers did not. Neither showed unambiguous vicarious attributions. Recall and attribution were again uncorrelated. These studies suggest that the imagery explanation of empathy effects is untenable, and imply that the recall of perspective-relevant details is unlikely to mediate attributions of causality in imaginary scenarios. 相似文献
53.
54.
M.S. Halliday 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1977,23(3):378-390
Three experiments are reported on behavioral inference in children of 3 to 5 years of age. In each experiment the children learned two separate sequences of behavior which they were required to put together in the final test stage in order to obtain a reward. In the first experiment moderate levels of behavioral inference were obtained, although a relatively high proportion of children failed to respond in the test phase. In the second experiment different instructions were used in the test phase, the number of nonresponders was greatly reduced and a much higher proportion of children showed behavioral inference. The third experiment obtained similar results under conditions which excluded the possibility of mediation during a memory test, which was included in the previous experiments. It was concluded that behavioral inference is found, under suitable conditions, in children as young as 3-years-old, and that reports by other investigators of failure to find such inference up to the age of about 8 were due to problems with apparatus and instructions. 相似文献
55.
Richard N Aslin 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1977,23(1):133-150
Two experiments measured changes in binocular eye alignment from 1- to 6-month-old human infants. In Experiment 1 changes in binocular eye alignment were recorded from 1, 2, and 3 month olds using corneal photography. A luminous target was moved along the infant's midline at one of two constant speeds. Infants at all three ages showed some evidence of appropriate changes in binocular eye alignment (convergence as the target approached and divergence as the target receded). The likelihood of appropriate convergence and divergence increased with age, as did the ability to respond appropriately to the faster target motion. A measure of convergence lag (further decrease in interpupillary distance after target motion had ended) was obtained for trials on which the target approached the infant. Convergence lag decreased with age and was greater on trials at the faster target speed until 3 months of age. Experiment 2 measured the responses of 3, , and 6 month olds as a wedge prism was placed alternately in front of each eye. The prism displaced the image nasally (either 2.5° or 5°), creating inappropriate binocular eye alignment. A refixation eye movement in response to the prism was not present consistently until 6 months of age. Hypotheses as to the mechanism controlling infant binocular eye alignment are discussed with respect both to the present findings and to findings from human adults and animals. 相似文献
56.
A series of four experiments is reported in which reaction times for naming line drawings were analyzed as a function of the similarity of the print superimposed on the drawings to the printed label for the drawing. This effect was studied as a function of grade level (ages 6, 8, and 11 years) and, hence, reading experience. The studies were designed to determine more precisely the characteristics of the stage of visual feature analysis of word identification and to examine response competition factors in this interference task. Results indicated that a combination of end letters and word shape was important for word identification and that response articulation was a significant distinct interference component in this task. 相似文献
57.
James M. Kauffman 《Journal of School Psychology》1977,15(4):343-348
A survey of several behavior modification journals showed that only 28% of the studies conducted in educational settings reported any follow-up data. Of those studies including follow-up data, 86% reported that therapeutic gains were completely or partially maintained. Follow-up was more often reported and was longer when the subject was an individual or small group than when an entire class served as the subject. More follow-up data were based on direct observation than on anecdotal verbal reports, but the follow-up period was longer when verbal reports were obtained than when direct observation was used. A greater percentage of studies published prior to 1971 than of those published since 1971 included follow-up data. Problems in the methodology and design of follow-up research are discussed. 相似文献
58.
Catherine G. Wolf 《Brain and language》1977,4(1):70-77
The temporal characteristics of auditory memory were investigated using a reaction time paradigm. The study attempted to determine whether or not there are functionally distinct left and right hemisphere components of auditory memory with different capacities for the retention of nonlinguistic information, such as fundamental frequency, over the course of several seconds. The results indicated that fundamental frequency information in auditory memory remains substantially unchanged over the course of 2 sec. No evidence was found for the existence of separate left and right hemisphere components of auditory memory with different temporal characteristics. The implications of this finding for the organization of auditory memory in the brain are discussed. 相似文献
59.
Vivian S Boyd 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1976,9(2):191-199
Sex-role stereotyping in the linguistic structure of Holland's Self-Directed Search (SDS) was examined. A revised SDS was constructed involving the removal of all masculine-toned terminology from the items and test-taking instructions, as well as the affixing of the letters M/F and F/M to all occupational titles and terms commonly perceived as gender specific. Subjects were 266 lower division female students at a large state university; 133 subjects completed the standard SDS and 133 completed the revised form. Following SDS assessment, subjects rated their inventory with respect to perceived sexual equality. Comparison variables included: daydreams, occupations' scores, summary codes, and subsequent occupational choices. Occupational choices at each stage of the assessment process were classified as nontraditional or traditional choices for women. Results indicated that across all indices derived from the standard and revised versions of the SDS, there were no significant differences in these subjects' performance. The subjects did perceive the two inventories differently, with subjects completing the standard SDS viewing it as slightly less equitable. 相似文献
60.