全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2481篇 |
免费 | 117篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
2605篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 34篇 |
2023年 | 64篇 |
2022年 | 39篇 |
2021年 | 123篇 |
2020年 | 224篇 |
2019年 | 260篇 |
2018年 | 224篇 |
2017年 | 199篇 |
2016年 | 140篇 |
2015年 | 121篇 |
2014年 | 92篇 |
2013年 | 524篇 |
2012年 | 49篇 |
2011年 | 42篇 |
2010年 | 23篇 |
2009年 | 39篇 |
2008年 | 36篇 |
2007年 | 40篇 |
2006年 | 48篇 |
2005年 | 35篇 |
2004年 | 33篇 |
2003年 | 24篇 |
2002年 | 15篇 |
2001年 | 21篇 |
2000年 | 16篇 |
1999年 | 11篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 18篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有2605条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
971.
Todd Lucas Cort Rudolph Ludmila Zhdanova Evone Barkho Nathan Weidner 《Political psychology》2014,35(6):775-793
Harsh treatment of others can reflect an underlying motivation to view the world as fair and just and also a dispositional tendency to believe in justice. However, there is a critical need to refine and expand existing knowledge, not only to identify underlying psychological processes but also to better understand how justice may be implicated in support for exclusionary policies. Across two studies, we show that support for policies that restrict immigrants is exclusively associated with thoughts about fair outcomes for other people (distributive justice for others). In Study 1, Americans' dispositional tendency to believe in distributive justice for others was associated with greater support for a policy proposing to further restrict immigrant job seekers' capacity to gain employment in the United States. In Study 2, we experimentally primed thoughts about justice in a sample of U.S. police officers. Support for a policy that mandated stricter policing of illegal immigration was strongest among officers who first thought about fair outcomes for other people, relative to other unique justice primes. Across both studies, distributive justice for others was associated with greater collective angst—perceived threat towards the future existence of Americans. Moreover, collective angst mediated the link between distributive justice for others and support for restrictive policies. Overall, this research suggests that thoughts about distributive justice for others can especially diminish compassion towards immigrants and other underprivileged groups via support for exclusionary policies. In addition, merely thinking about distributive justice for others may be sufficient to amplify social callousness. 相似文献
972.
《International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology》2014,14(1):83-86
Many articles published in the International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology describe the results obtained in studies that have used Likert-type scales. Such scales have advantages and disadvantages and in this article I comment on some of these. In particular I comment on the effects of using scales that start with the positive end and finish with the negative one, and on those that present high ratings on the left - as opposed to the right. I also consider that scores on negatively-worded items differ from those obtained on positively-worded ones and that it is not, therefore, appropriate to simply reverse the scores obtained on such items. Finally, I note that some items on these scales present more than one issue and it is hard to know whether when responding to these items, respondents are responding to all of the item or just one part. 相似文献
973.
《Revue Européene de Psychologie Appliquée》2014,64(2):43-51
IntroductionControlled and autonomous motivational factors from self-determination theory have previously been highlighted as key factors in eating regulation. The present study examined controlled motives as an overarching motivational factor in eating dysregulation and examined its effects on dieting behaviour for those who are underweight, overweight or obese.ObjectiveTo examine whether the influence of controlled motives on dieting behaviour would be moderated by body mass index (BMI). Specifically, it was hypothesised that controlled motives would be associated with high levels of dieting behaviour in underweight individuals and low levels of dieting behaviour in individuals classified as overweight or obese.MethodOne hundred and thirty-seven participants completed the measures of height, weight, and motivation and intentions towards watching their diet and subsequently completed a measure of dieting behaviour two weeks later.ResultsModerated regression and simple slopes analyses provided support for the hypothesised effects at underweight, overweight, and obese range BMIs.ConclusionThe effect of controlled motives on dieting behaviour is dependent upon BMI and therefore varies across underweight, overweight, and obese individuals. The implications of controlled motives and external pressures to watch one's diet are discussed. 相似文献
974.
该研究采用艾尔斯伯格悖论的变式,选取二择一的迫选任务,旨在探讨不同球数和不同模糊程度下模糊决策的决策偏好。结果发现,球数的不同对决策的偏好基本上没有影响;而模糊程度的不同,对决策的偏好有影响。具体表现为:模糊程度较高时,被试倾向于模糊厌恶,模糊程度较低时倾向于模糊寻求。 相似文献
975.
《Estudios de Psicología》2013,34(1):61-86
ResumenEste artículo presenta algunos modelos de relación entre tiempo y explicación inductiva. Sobre esta base el tiempo se define como sucesión y la explicación inductiva se restringe a vinculaciones entre eventos sucesivos. Analizamos esta relación desde el punto de vista epistemológico, dibujando algunos patrones triádicos inspirados en la pragmàtica de Peirce, a fin de conectar la explicación causal con las ideas de sucesión, cambio, movimiento, organización y conducta. Examinamos estos patrones en las Ciencias Naturales y Sociales y los valoramos a la luz de los debates sobre causalidad, centrándonos en la amplia banda de elecciones entre descripción y explicación y distinguiendo entre interpretaciones secuenciales orientadas al pasado o al futuro. En este contexto de análisis representamos el núcleo de una interpretación secuencial inductiva como una tríada “causa-conducta/acción-función”, que apunta tanto a antecedentes como a efectos. Finalmente, proyectamos este modelo en el nivel semiótico de la interacción y la cultura. 相似文献
976.
《Estudios de Psicología》2013,34(3):385-396
ResumenEl objetivo principal de este trabajo es explorar los procesos de categorización del yo en niveles subordinados y supraordenados en contextos organizacionales y sus antecedentes y resultados actitudinales y conductuales. Dos perfiles diferentes de identificación social en una muestra de profesores de primaria se han propuesto (N = 119), la orientación hacia el subgrupo y la orientación global. Usando modelos de ecuaciones estructurales los resultados muestran que el tamaño del subgrupo, el contacto del subgrupo, la discriminación percibida y la competición entre grupos son predictores relevantes de los perfiles de identificación social. Más aún, la orientación hacia el subgrupo predice el favoritismo endogrupal mientras la orientación global predice las conductas de ciudadanía globales. 相似文献
977.
《European Journal of Developmental Psychology》2013,10(4):452-472
This study investigated the relationship between different components of national identities of immigrants (related to both the country of origin and host country), and psychosocial adjustment in pre-migration and post-migration periods. Adolescents who immigrated from Russia and Ukraine to Israel (N = 151) completed questionnaires at four time points: about six months prior to, and in the first, second, and third years after, immigration. At each point, adolescents were assessed on degree of identification they felt with the citizens of their country of origin and Israel and on their attitudes towards the two countries. Five indexes of psychosocial adjustment were measured: emotional and behavioural problems, self-esteem, social competence, school competence and loneliness. Adolescents' positive attitudes towards the host country were associated with higher psychosocial adjustment, both before and after immigration. Positive attitudes towards the country of origin were associated with higher psychosocial adjustment in the post-migration period. Degree of identification with either nation was not related to psychosocial adjustment. Findings suggest that it may not be identity per se that is the critical factor for adjustment for adolescent immigrants, but rather the extent to which they feel affirmation, pride and positive regard towards the two countries. For adolescent immigrants, idealization of origin and host countries may fulfil an important developmental role allowing them to de-idealize parental figures and create new objects of idealization. 相似文献
978.
Mitchell J. Callan Aaron C. Kay John H. Ellard 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2009,45(4):614-623
We examined whether people might distort and selectively remember the past in ways that enable them to sustain a belief in a just world (BJW; Lerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion. New York: Plenum Press). In Study 1, recall of a lottery prize reflected participants’ justice concerns, such that the average lottery amount recalled was lowest when a “bad” versus “good” person won. In Study 2, an unrelated experience of just world threat (versus affirmation) enhanced biased recall of the lottery prize when the winner was undeserving. In Study 3, participants who experienced a fortuitous bad break selectively remembered more bad deeds from their recent past, whereas participants who experienced a good break selectively remembered more good deeds. Study 4 demonstrates that such selective memory biases specifically serve to portray chance outcomes as more fair. Taken together, these findings offer support for the notion that reconstructing and selectively recalling the past can serve to sustain a BJW. 相似文献
979.
This study is designed to examine how biological knowledge is organized and structured in children. Five- to nine-year-old children's interpretations of biological phenomena are studied by examining to what extent the transmission of different kinds of properties is perceived as being causally related to birth origins. A property generalization task is used, in which biological relations (birth origins) are pitted against adoptive relations. The characteristics to be generalized correspond to non-obvious biological characteristics, visible physic characteristics or behavioural characteristics. Our results show that five-year-old children do not understand the origin of these characteristics. A conceptual change seems to be at work by the age of seven but the delimitation of biological categories remains imperfect. 相似文献
980.
One of the major functions of academic journals is to contribute to the evaluation of research activities and scientists. Invented more than 50 years ago, the ISI impact factor (IF) became the most important indicator of the quality of journals, in spite of well-known problems and critics such as the over-representation of English-language journals. This is a specific problem for French publishers and scientists; publishing in French is not valorising. Since 2007, the new SCImago Journal Rank Indicator (SJR) offers an alternative to the IF. SJR applies the Google algorithm (PageRank) to the journals of the SCOPUS bibliographic database that indexes more journals than ISI Web of Science. The goal of our study is to compare the two indicators for French academic journals, with three questions: Which is the coverage of French journals by ISI and SCOPUS (title number, scientific disciplines)? Which are the differences of the two indicators IF and SJR for the ranking of French journals? How do they cover the French academic journal publishing market (representativity)? The results of our study of 368 French journals with IF and/or SJR are in favour of the usage of the new indicator, at least as a complement to the IF. (1) Coverage: 166 journals are indexed by ISI (45%), 345 journals are indexed by SCOPUS (94%), 143 journals are indexed by both (39%). 82% of the journals are from STM, 18% are from SS&H. In particular, SCOPUS covers much better the medical and pharmacological sciences. (2) Ranking: The correlation of IF and SJR for the 143 journals with both indicators is high (0.76). The IF better differentiates the journals than the SJR indicator (155 vs. 89 rankings). On the other side, because of the larger source database, more French titles become visible on an international level through SJR than through IF. (3) Representativity: The SJR is more interesting and representative of the French academic journal publishing market than the IF (19% vs. 9%), especially for STM titles (38% vs. 19%), much less for SS&H titles (6% vs. 2%). Nevertheless, ISI (Web of Science) and SCOPUS index journals from only a small part of the French academic publishers (10%–20%). Again, SCOPUS is more representative than the ISI dababase (17% of the publishers vs. 10%). Methodological problems and perspective of a multidimensional evaluation are discussed. Our study compares the ISI impact factor (IF) with the new SJR for 368 French academic journals with IF and/or SJR. The results: The SJR coverage is better than of the IF (94% vs. 45%), especially in medical sciences. The correlation of IF and SJR for journals with both indicators is high (0.76). The IF better differentiates the journals than the SJR indicator (155 vs. 89 rankings). The SJR coverage is more representative of the French academic journal publishing market than ISI/IF (19% vs. 9%), especially for STM titles (38% vs. 19%), less for SS&H titles (6% vs. 2%). 相似文献