首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   458篇
  免费   131篇
  国内免费   104篇
  2023年   8篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   12篇
  2020年   34篇
  2019年   24篇
  2018年   48篇
  2017年   18篇
  2016年   30篇
  2015年   25篇
  2014年   34篇
  2013年   64篇
  2012年   30篇
  2011年   26篇
  2010年   19篇
  2009年   23篇
  2008年   17篇
  2007年   17篇
  2006年   30篇
  2005年   28篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   20篇
  2002年   17篇
  2001年   15篇
  2000年   12篇
  1999年   12篇
  1998年   12篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   5篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   7篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   6篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   5篇
  1975年   4篇
排序方式: 共有693条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
11.
We investigated the possibility that human-like fixed-interval performances would appear in rats given a variable-ratio history (Wanchisen, Tatham, & Mooney, 1989). Nine rats were trained under single or compound variable-ratio schedules and then under a fixed-interval 30-s schedule. The histories produced high fixed-interval rates that declined slowly over 90 sessions; differences as a function of the particular history were absent. Nine control animals given only fixed-interval training responded at lower levels initially, but rates increased with training. Despite differences in absolute rates, rates within the intervals and postreinforcement pauses indicated equivalent development of the accelerated response patterns suggestive of sensitivity to fixed-interval contingencies. The finding that the histories elevated rates without retarding development of differentiated patterns suggests that the effective response unit was a burst of several lever presses and that the fixed-interval contingencies acted on these units in the same way as for single responses. Regardless of history, the rats did not manifest the persistent, undifferentiated responding reported for humans under comparable schedules. We concluded that the shortcomings of animal models of human fixed-interval performances cannot be easily remedied by including a variable-ratio conditioning history within the model.  相似文献   
12.
Biasing the pacemaker in the behavioral theory of timing   总被引:2,自引:2,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
In the behavioral theory of timing, pacemaker rate is determined by overall rate of reinforcement. A two-alternative free-operant psychophysical procedure was employed to investigate whether pacemaker period was also sensitive to the differential rate of reinforcement. Responding on a left key during the first 25 s and on a right key during the second 25 s of a 50-s trial was reinforced at variable intervals, and variable-interval schedule values during the two halves of the trials were varied systematically. Responding on the right key during the first 25 s and on the left key during the second 25 s was not reinforced. Estimates of pacemaker period were derived from fits of a function predicted by the behavioral theory of timing to right-key response proportions in consecutive 5-s bins of the 50-s trial. Estimates of pacemaker period were shortest when the differential reinforcer rate most strongly favored right-key responses, and were longest when the differential reinforcer rate most strongly favored left-key responses. The results were consistent with the conclusion that pacemaker rate is influenced by relative reinforcer rate.  相似文献   
13.
14.
大学生场依存性─-独立性认知方式分析   总被引:14,自引:0,他引:14  
对二十一个学科专业1526名大学生的场依存性─-独立性认知方式进行调查所获得的新近常模资料表明:大学生场依存性─-独立性个体差异的群体分布是正态性的,其总体平均水平以《认知方式图形测验》成绩作指标为13.7(s为4.6)。在此基础上,对文献所报道的影响场依存性─-独立性发展水平的两个因素作了进一步的研究分析,发现,大学生场依存性─-独立性的性别差异非常接近显著水平;专业分化是不依赖于性别因素而导致大学生场依存性─-独立性水平显著不同的主要因素.  相似文献   
15.
未来时间的心理结构   总被引:38,自引:5,他引:33  
黄希庭 《心理学报》1994,27(2):121-127
220名大学生被试用秒、分、小时、日、星期、月和年7个时间单位和模糊统计试验方法对39个不定的未来时间修饰词作经验赋值,结果表明:①39个不定的未来时间修饰词与秒、分、小时、日、月、年有一定的对应关系,②未来的心理时间分为三个时间段,即以秒和分为计时单位的“较近的未来”,以小时、日和月为计时单位的“近的未来”,以及以年为计时单位的“远的未来”。③离现在越近的未来时间修饰词,词义模糊度越小,评量的把握度越大;而离现在越远的未来时间修饰词,词义模糊度越大,评量的把握度越小。  相似文献   
16.
Operant temporal discrimination learning was investigated in goldfish. In the first experiment, there was a fixed daily change in illumination. Eight subjects were trained to operate a lever that reinforced each press with food. The period during which responses were reinforced was then progressively reduced until it was 1 hr in every 24. The final 1-hr feeding schedule was maintained over 4 weeks. The feeding period commenced at the same time each day throughout. The food dispensers were then made inactive, and a period of extinction ensued for 6 days. The pattern of responding suggested that the fish were able to exhibit temporal discrimination in anticipation of feeding time. This pattern of responding persisted for a limited number of days during the extinction procedure. The second experiment produced evidence that operant temporal discrimination could develop under continuous illumination.  相似文献   
17.
系列范畴词表时序记忆和项目记忆研究   总被引:3,自引:1,他引:2  
杨志新 《心理学报》1998,31(1):35-42
该项研究探讨系列范畴词表的时序长时记忆和项目长时记忆,实验设计的系列范畴词表将记忆项目分为词表内范畴相同项和词表间范畴相同项,时序记记测量方法是词表辨认,项目记忆测量方法是自由回忆。实验一发现:2分钟延缓测试词表内范畴相同项时序记忆和项目记忆优于词表间范畴相同项;6分钟以及10分钟延缓测试时序记忆仍是词表内范畴相同项好。实验二发现:以范畴线索提取,两类项目记忆没有显著差异;以词表线索提取,两类项目  相似文献   
18.
时序信息提取机制的探索   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
李宏翰  黄希庭 《心理学报》1996,29(2):180-191
对时序信息加工的经典研究是采用新近性判断范型,结果发现其提取机制是以新近性为基础的逆向串行搜索过程。本研究采用新近性判断范型和早远性判断范型对时序信息的提取机制进行了深入的考察,结果表明:(l)提取时序信息既存在逆向串行搜索,又存在顺向串行搜索;(2)早远性判断和新近性判断任务对不同部分时序信息恢复的效应不同,其中早远性判断易化早远部分,新近性判断易化新近部分──表现为对相应部分辨别力的提高和正确反应潜伏期的缩短;(3)在不同的时序信息提取任务中,被试会根据具体条件进行反转反应。  相似文献   
19.
Temporal control of behavior: schedule interactions   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
In Experiment I the response that terminated the postreinforcement pauses occurring under a fixed-interval 60-second schedule was reinforced, if the pause duration exceeded 30 seconds. The percentage of such pauses, rather than increasing, decreased. There were complex effects on the discriminative control of the pause by the reinforcer terminating the previous fixed interval, depending on whether the fixed interval and the added reinforcer were the same or different. In Experiments II(a) and II(b), each reinforcement initiated an alternative fixed-interval interresponse-time-greater-than-t-sec schedule, the schedule values being systematically varied. When the response following a pause exceeding a given duration was reinforced, fewer such pauses occurred than when they were not reinforced, i.e., on the comparable simple fixed-interval schedule. There was no systematic relationship between mean interrinforcement interval and duration of the postreinforcement pause. The pause duration initiated by reinforcement was directly related to the dependency controlling the shortest pause at that time, regardless of changes in mean interreinforcement interval.  相似文献   
20.
Temporal control of behavior and the power law   总被引:9,自引:9,他引:0       下载免费PDF全文
The performance of rats and pigeons under fixed-interval schedules was studied in two experiments. The duration of postreinforcement pause was a declining proportion of fixed-interval duration. For pigeons this was true both when the duration of the reinforcer was fixed and when it was increased in direct proportion to increases in fixed-interval duration; the longer reinforcer durations did, however, lengthen the postreinforcement pause at higher schedule values. A quantitative analysis of data from Experiments 1 and 2 and from other studies showed that fractional exponent power functions described the relationship between postreinforcement pause and fixed-interval value; similar functions have previously been observed in studies of temporal differentiation. It was concluded that power functions reflect a direct causal, rather than artifactual, relationship between performance and the temporal requirements of reinforcement schedules.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号