全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2014篇 |
免费 | 176篇 |
国内免费 | 82篇 |
专业分类
2272篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 71篇 |
2022年 | 33篇 |
2021年 | 70篇 |
2020年 | 117篇 |
2019年 | 132篇 |
2018年 | 87篇 |
2017年 | 95篇 |
2016年 | 81篇 |
2015年 | 63篇 |
2014年 | 63篇 |
2013年 | 248篇 |
2012年 | 49篇 |
2011年 | 59篇 |
2010年 | 61篇 |
2009年 | 78篇 |
2008年 | 60篇 |
2007年 | 78篇 |
2006年 | 69篇 |
2005年 | 77篇 |
2004年 | 65篇 |
2003年 | 48篇 |
2002年 | 57篇 |
2001年 | 53篇 |
2000年 | 39篇 |
1999年 | 43篇 |
1998年 | 32篇 |
1997年 | 36篇 |
1996年 | 28篇 |
1995年 | 31篇 |
1994年 | 29篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 15篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 18篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有2272条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
91.
Elyssa H. Gerst Paul T. Cirino Jack M. Fletcher Hanako Yoshida 《Child neuropsychology》2017,23(4):381-407
Interrelations of two measurement methods (cognitive versus behavioral ratings) for executive function (EF) were examined and related to reading comprehension and math calculations in fourth and fifth grade students (n = 93) in the context of a diverse urban student population. Relations among measures within four EF processes (working memory, planning, inhibition and shifting) were modest; relations to academics were stronger. EF measures contributed to both academic outcomes even in the context of relevant covariates (age, language and educational program). Working memory was particularly important for reading comprehension across measurement type. Cognitive measures from all EF processes, particularly inhibition and planning, and behavioral ratings of working memory were important for math. 相似文献
92.
《Journal of Investigative Psychology & Offender Profiling》2017,14(1):3-22
The aim of the current vignette study is to map the style, type, and themes of questions that are asked when assessing the credibility of asylum seekers' claims. Sixty‐five officials from the Swedish Migration Agency (Migrationsverket ), were asked to respond to one out of four different vignettes that contained fictitious asylum narratives. Each vignette presented the types of problems often encountered by officials at the migration board. Two of the vignettes contained no evidence of the origin of the asylum seeker. The other two contained no evidence for the claim of persecution. The asylum officials were asked to formulate five questions that would help them to assess the veracity of the applicant's claim. Our analyses showed that they mainly formulated open questions in an information gathering style. A thematic analysis of the questions revealed that when a claim about origin was assessed, asylum officials mostly asked questions about life in the country of origin, identity documents, and the flight to Europe. When the claim about persecution was assessed, in contrast, asylum officials mostly formulated case‐specific questions (e.g., how the applicant was arrested). Hence, when the credibility of claims about origin is assessed, there seems to be a typical set of questions that asylum officials use. The asylum officials seem to assume that if the applicant is truly originating from a specific country or area, he or she should have ample knowledge about that area, its customs, and frequently encountered objects. 相似文献
93.
94.
We applied the traits as density distributions of states approach to generalized expectations of trustworthiness, namely, social trust and trust in politicians. Using an experience sampling study (N = 47), we assessed state social trust and trust in politicians four times a day for 2 weeks. Within-person variability was found to be low but meaningful as it was predicted by variations in affect, interactional trust, and prior experiences. There was high stability in interindividual differences in the mean levels of state trustworthiness expectations and in the levels of within-person variability. Our study provides a comprehensive understanding of stability and variability in generalized expectations of trustworthiness and indicates broad applicability of the traits as density distributions of states approach. 相似文献
95.
Lindauer SE Zarcone JR Richman DM Schroeder SR 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2002,35(3):299-303
Functional analysis results indicated that maladaptive behaviors displayed by a 25-year-old man with profound mental retardation were maintained by attention from caregivers and possibly, to a lesser degree, by access to tangible items. A concurrent-schedules procedure was then used to study the relative reinforcing value for maladaptive behavior of attention versus tangible items. Results of the concurrent-schedules assessment and subsequent functional communication training indicated that preference for attention versus access to a tangible object varied. 相似文献
96.
An evaluation of food type and texture indicated that both variables affected the expulsions of a 3-year-old with feeding problems. The results of the evaluation were used to prescribe a treatment (reducing the texture of one food type) that reduced expulsion. 相似文献
97.
The experiments described in the lead articles replicate findings from previous studies of development of knowledge about balance scales, add several new findings, and raise four key questions: (a) How can rule use best be assessed? (b) How can we reconcile systematic use of rules with variable use of strategies? (c) When do children begin to use rules? and (d) How do children generate new rules? In this Reflection, we summarize current understanding of development of knowledge about balance scales and consider each of the four questions. 相似文献
98.
The Dental Subscale of the Children's Fear Survey Schedule: Predictive Value and Clinical Usefulness
M. Ten Berge J. S. J. Veerkamp J. Hoogstraten P. J. M. Prins 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2002,24(2):115-118
This study reports on the predictive value and clinical usefulness of the Dutch parental version of the Dental Subscale of the Children's Fear Survey Schedule (CFSS-DS). Parents of 718 children (4–12 years) completed this CFSS-DS version before or during the child's visit. The dentist rated the child's dental fear during treatment on a 5-point Likert-type Scale from 1 (not afraid at all) to 5 (very afraid). Reliability analysis was performed, and correlation coefficients between the two measures were calculated. The reliability of the scale proved to be high (Cronbach's = .93) and significant correlation coefficients were found (r = .58 and r = .68, p < .01). The negative predictive value of the CFSS-DS was high (.96), whereas its positive predictive value was relatively low (0.41). It was concluded that the CFSS-DS might be of clinical value as a screening device of dental fear, whereas its predictive value of fearful behavior should not be overestimated. 相似文献
99.
The aim of the present study was to test the properties of an instrument that assesses concerns about weight and eating without reference to dieting behavior. A short instrument, the Weight and Eating Concerns Inventory (WECI) was examined in a sample of 569 boys and 548 girls aged 11 to 15 years. Confirmatory factor analyses with LISREL showed a better fit with a version of the instrument that did not include a reference to dieting behavior, compared to a version that included such a reference. This was true for both boys and girls, irrespective of their age group. However, the results indicated that both versions should be used with caution for young boys. The internal consistency of the WECI (that is, the version without reference to dieting) was satisfactory, ranging from 0.78 to 0.86 for girls and from 0.68 to 0.73 for boys. The WECI correlated quite substantially with negative self-evaluations and depression for boys and girls in all the age groups assessed, suggesting that high scores on the WECI may indicate a problem that goes beyond worries or concerns, and should be taken seriously. In general, girls reported more of these concerns than boys, and the correlation between the WECI and dieting was stronger among girls compared to boys. 相似文献
100.
Tangible preference assessments were compared with verbal preference assessments for 6 individuals with mental retardation, behavior disorders, or both. In the tangible assessment, items were placed in front of the participant. In the verbal assessment, participants were asked, “Do you want X or Y?” and the items were not present. The two assessments yielded similar high‐preference items for 4 of the 6 participants. The verbal assessment was typically completed in less time than the tangible assessment. 相似文献