全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1431篇 |
免费 | 223篇 |
国内免费 | 58篇 |
专业分类
1712篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 6篇 |
2024年 | 21篇 |
2023年 | 20篇 |
2022年 | 38篇 |
2021年 | 49篇 |
2020年 | 83篇 |
2019年 | 84篇 |
2018年 | 79篇 |
2017年 | 88篇 |
2016年 | 97篇 |
2015年 | 45篇 |
2014年 | 77篇 |
2013年 | 286篇 |
2012年 | 70篇 |
2011年 | 80篇 |
2010年 | 41篇 |
2009年 | 56篇 |
2008年 | 59篇 |
2007年 | 64篇 |
2006年 | 38篇 |
2005年 | 49篇 |
2004年 | 41篇 |
2003年 | 27篇 |
2002年 | 38篇 |
2001年 | 25篇 |
2000年 | 16篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有1712条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
971.
Three studies examined the effects of attachment style on performance in non-attachment-related attention tasks; one study also assessed the effect of priming memories of experiences of attachment security or insecurity on attentional performance in a flanker task. In Study 1, participants performed a psychological refractory period (PRP) task assessing their ability to switch attention rapidly from one decision to another; in Studies 2 and 3 they performed a flanker task assessing their ability to resist distracters. Avoidant attachment predicted better performance on both tasks, and the effects remained even after controlling for neuroticism, general anxiety, and BIS/BAS scores. Study 3 showed that thinking in detail about a past experience of insecurity eliminated avoidant participants’ superior performance. In sum, avoidant people are generally skilled at regulating their attention, even on non-attachment-related tasks, but their performance is hampered by reminders of experiences of insecurity. 相似文献
972.
Extending Fitts' law to a three-dimensional pointing task 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
An attempt was made to extend Fitts' law to a three-dimensional movement (pointing) task to enhance its predictive performance in this domain. An experiment was conducted in which 10 subjects performed three-dimensional pointing movements under the manipulation of target size, distance to target and direction to target. As expected, the duration of these three-dimensional movements was rather variable and affected markedly by direction to target. As a result, the variance in the movement times produced was not satisfactorily explained by the conventional Fitts' model. The conventional model was extended by incorporating a directional parameter into the model. The extended model was shown to better fit the data than the conventional Fitts' model, both in terms of r(2) and the standard error of the residual between the measured movement time and the value predicted by model fit. 相似文献
973.
采用任务转换范式,分别以阿拉伯数字奇偶判断和组成汉字词的汉字结构一致性判断为任务,通过两个实验探查了不同类型材料在任务转换中的反应—反应相容性效应。结果发现:(1)材料类型对反应—反应相容性效应有影响,任务转换前阿拉伯数字材料存在反应—反应相容性效应,汉字词材料不存在反应—反应相容性效应;(2)任务转换时段对阿拉伯数字奇偶判断任务的反应—反应相容性效应没有影响,转换前后阿拉伯数字奇偶判断任务都存在反应—反应相容性效应;(3)任务转换时段对汉字词任务的反应—反应相容性有影响。转换前没有反应—反应相容性效应,转换后存在反应—反应相容性效应。 相似文献
974.
Kenneth J. Dunegan Mary Uhl-Bien Dennis Duchon 《Journal of business and psychology》2002,17(2):275-285
Role conflict, role ambiguity, and intrinsic task satisfaction are found to moderate the relationship between leader–member exchange (LMX) and subordinate performance. Data from a field study of 146 supervisor–subordinate dyads indicate low conflict, high ambiguity, and high intrinsic satisfaction enhance the link between LMX and performance. Neutralizing effects are found when ambiguity and intrinsic satisfaction are low. High conflict appears to have a constraining effect, whereby the connection between LMX and performance is reduced but not neutralized. Results from the study call attention to the theoretical and practical benefits of examining the LMX/performance link from a contingency perspective, and offer a viable, albeit tentative, explanation for inconsistent findings reported in earlier studies. 相似文献
975.
The model of a single central bottleneck for human information processing is critically examined. Most evidence cited in support of the model has been observed within the overlapping tasks paradigm. It is shown here that most findings obtained within that paradigm and that were used to support the model are also consistent with a simple resource model. The most prominent findings are the millisecond-for-millisecond slope at the left of the RT2-SOA curve, the high RT1-RT2 correlation, the additivity of the effects on RT2 of SOA and of the difficulty of selecting R2, and the washout of the effect of S2 discriminability on RT2 in a dual-task condition. In addition, the asymmetry of the effects of the dual-task requirement on RT1 and RT2 can be accounted for by the resource model provided that it assumes uneven allocation of resources, which is quite reasonable in view of the task asymmetry inherent in the demand characteristics of the paradigm. The same is true for two other findings that appear to support the single-bottleneck model-that in the dual-task condition, the demand of the first task affects equally RT1 and RT2 and that its effect on RT1 is the same as the corresponding effect in the single-task condition. Furthermore, the single-bottleneck model is hard to reconcile with a negative slope at the left of the RT1-SOA curve or a positive slope at the left of the IRI-SOA curve, unless augmented by ancillary assumptions that are yet to be substantiated. Representative data were fit by each of the models using its optimal set of parameters. Both models achieved quite good degrees of fit. It is further argued that since the overlapping tasks paradigm is heavily biased in favor of a speedy reaction to the stimulus that appears first, it is nonoptimal for testing the central bottleneck model. Finally, the bottleneck model is examined in terms of other scientific criteria. 相似文献
976.
本研究采用RSVP任务考察了在汉字信息加工中间隔效应对重复知盲效应的影响。研究发现:(1)间隔效应是影响重复知盲效应的主要因素之一。当间隔数较小(或间隔时间较短)时,有较强的重复知盲效应;随着间隔数的增加(或间隔时间的延长),重复知盲效应逐渐减弱;(2)在同一系列位置,第一个重复项R1的正确率远大于第二个重复项R2,这可能反映出注意在重复知盲效应中的作用。 相似文献
977.
Deficits in working memory have been shown to contribute to poor performance on the Iowa Gambling Task [IGT: Bechara, A., &; Martin, E.M. (2004). Impaired decision making related to working memory deficits in individuals with substance addictions. Neuropsychology, 18, 152–162]. Similarly, a secondary memory load task has been shown to impair task performance [Hinson, J., Jameson, T. &; Whitney, P. (2002). Somatic markers, working memory, and decision making. Cognitive, Affective, &; Behavioural Neuroscience, 2, 341–353]. In the present study, we investigate whether the latter findings were due to increased random responding [Franco-Watkins, A. M., Pashler, H., &; Rickard, T. C. (2006). Does working memory load lead to greater impulsivity? Commentary on Hinson, Jameson, and Whitney’s (2003). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory &; Cognition, 32, 443–447]. Participants were tested under Low Working Memory (LWM; n = 18) or High Working Memory (HWM; n = 17) conditions while performing the Reversed IGT in which punishment was immediate and reward delayed [Bechara, A., Dolan, S., &; Hindes, A. (2002). Decision making and addiction (part II): Myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Neuropsychologia, 40, 1690–1705]. In support of a role for working memory in emotional decision making, compared to the LWM condition, participants in the HWM condition made significantly greater number of disadvantageous selections than that predicted by chance. Performance by the HWM group could not be fully explained by random responding. 相似文献
978.
This event-related potential study investigated (i) to what extent incongruence between attention-directing cue and cued target modality affects attentional control processes that bias the system in advance to favor a particular stimulus modality and (ii) to what extent top-down attentional control mechanisms are generalized for the type of information that is to be attended. To this end, both visual and auditory word cues were used to instruct participants to direct attention to a specific visual (color) or auditory (pitch) stimulus feature of a forthcoming multisensory target stimulus. Effects of cue congruency were observed within 200 ms post-cue over frontal scalp regions and related to processes involved in shifting attention from the cue modality to the modality of the task-relevant target feature. Both directing visual attention and directing auditory attention were associated with dorsal posterior positivity, followed by sustained fronto-central negativity. However, this fronto-central negativity appeared to have an earlier onset and was more pronounced when the visual modality was cued. Together the present results suggest that the mechanisms involved in deploying attention are to some extent determined by the modality (visual, auditory) in which attention operates, and in addition, that some of these mechanisms can also be affected by cue congruency. 相似文献
979.
医患和谐是医患双方的共同需要,通过时单病种乳腺癌改良根治术等10个单病种临床路径与成本控制绩效分析,评价患者的满意效果。收集整理2006年7月至2007年12月10个单病种的统计信息,以其手术并发症发生率、平均住院天数、平均住院费用、患者满意率等作为评价指标,将实验组(应用临床路径和成本控制组)和对照组(未应用临床路径和成本控制组)进行对比分析。结果显示,应用临床路径和成本控制的患者与未应用这一方式的患者之间在手术并发症发生率、平均住院时间、平均住院费用、患者满意率方面有统计学显著差异。提示单病种临床路径和成本控制模式应用于临床管理,可以明显促进CQI(Continuous Quality Improvement持续性质量改进),缩短平均住院时间,降低住院费用,提高患者满意率,并从根本上改善医患关系。 相似文献
980.
Sadonya?F.?Meadows Christopher?H.?SkinnerEmail author 《Journal of Behavioral Education》2005,14(4):227-247
Research on the additive interspersal procedure was extended by exposing seventh-grade students to curricula-based (e.g.,
educationally valid) language arts assignments. In Experiment I, each student was given a control language arts assignment
containing 20 discrete target items and an experimental assignment containing 24 equivalent target items, plus eight interspersed
briefer items. After working on both assignments for 10 minutes, significantly more students chose a new experimental assignment
for homework. Individual analysis showed that 85% of the students preferred or chose the assignment associated with higher
discrete task completion rates. In Experiment II, students completed both assignments. Results extended previous research
by showing that even after expending more effort to complete the 20% more target items plus the additional interspersed items
on the experimental assignments, significantly more students chose an experimental assignment for homework. Applied and theoretical
implications are discussed along with limitations of the current study and directions for future research. 相似文献