全文获取类型
收费全文 | 551篇 |
免费 | 351篇 |
国内免费 | 41篇 |
专业分类
943篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 25篇 |
2020年 | 27篇 |
2019年 | 25篇 |
2018年 | 24篇 |
2017年 | 22篇 |
2016年 | 29篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 22篇 |
2013年 | 47篇 |
2012年 | 27篇 |
2011年 | 35篇 |
2010年 | 28篇 |
2009年 | 25篇 |
2008年 | 24篇 |
2007年 | 18篇 |
2006年 | 18篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 24篇 |
2003年 | 18篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 19篇 |
2000年 | 22篇 |
1999年 | 17篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 15篇 |
1996年 | 17篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 17篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 18篇 |
1978年 | 19篇 |
1977年 | 22篇 |
1976年 | 14篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有943条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
Taravella CC Lerman DC Contrucci SA Roane HS 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2000,33(1):105-108
The generality of the findings reported by DeLeon, Iwata, and Roscoe (1997) was examined by conducting two stimulus-choice preference assessments, the second of which evaluated low-ranked items from the initial assessment. Results for the 2 participants suggested that supplementary assessments of low-ranked items may be useful for identifying a wider variety of reinforcing stimuli. 相似文献
42.
Four homing pigeons were trained over 5 months in a zero-delay, “arbitrary” matching-to-sample procedure with sample and comparison stimuli presented on any of three response keys. Birds were also required to complete a fixed-ratio 10 requirement on both sample and comparison stimuli to terminate their presentation. The procedure resulted in the establishment of relations that were not specifically trained and that can be characterized by the property of transitivity in a stimulus equivalence context. This result was in contrast with the findings obtained from most previous research with nonhuman subjects. 相似文献
43.
The transfer of respondent eliciting and extinction functions through stimulus equivalence classes 下载免费PDF全文
Dougher MJ Augustson E Markham MR Greenway DE Wulfert E 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1994,62(3):331-351
Two studies investigated the transfer of respondent elicitation through equivalence classes. In Experiment 1, match-to-sample procedures were used to teach 8 subjects two four-member equivalence classes. One member of one class was then paired with electric shock, and one member of the other class was presented without shock. All remaining stimuli were then presented. Using skin conductance as the measure of conditioning, transfer of conditioning was demonstrated in 6 of the 8 subjects. In Experiment 2, similar procedures were used to replicate the results of Experiment 1 and investigate the transfer of extinction. Following equivalence training and conditioning to all members of one class, one member was then presented in extinction. When the remaining stimuli from this class were then presented, they failed to elicit skin conductance. In the final phase of the experiment, the stimulus that was previously presented in extinction was reconditioned. Test trials with other members of the class revealed that they regained elicitation function. These results demonstrate that both respondent elicitation and extinction can transfer through stimulus classes. The clinical and applied significance of the results is discussed. 相似文献
44.
Megan R. Heinicke James E. Carr Catherine J. Copsey 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2019,52(3):847-869
The purpose of this systematic review was to identify investigations comparing the efficacy of alternative modality (e.g., pictorial, verbal, video) stimulus preference assessments for individuals with developmental disabilities. We identified articles by searching peer‐reviewed journals using the PsycINFO and ERIC databases, conducting table of contents searches of common behavioral outlets, and conducting ancestral searches of recent reviews and practitioner summaries of preference assessment methodology. A total of 32 articles met our inclusion criteria. These studies were then coded across a variety of features to gain a better understanding of the efficacy of alternative format preference assessments for individuals with developmental disabilities. In addition, we reviewed this literature for the use of prerequisite‐skill assessments and contingent‐reinforcer access to further investigate the relation between these variables and the accuracy of pictorial, verbal, and video preference assessments. A variety of methodological concerns are discussed as well as suggestions for future research. 相似文献
45.
Mary Hunter Jesús Rosales‐Ruiz 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2019,111(3):449-464
During shaping, if the organism is engaged in behaviors other than the current approximation, the amount of time between reinforcers increases. In these situations, the shaper may resort to what is referred to as a “desperation‐driven click.” That is, after a period of no reinforcement, the shaper delivers one reinforcer for a nontarget approximation. Reports from professional animal trainers suggest that the animal may continue performing this new behavior, even if it is reinforced only once. This study attempted to model this phenomenon with college students. Results from the study demonstrated that a desperation‐driven click situation can be reliably produced in a controlled setting. When participants received one reinforcer for interacting with a new object following a period of no reinforcement, they interacted with the new object for a longer or equal amount of time as compared to an object that had a longer history of reinforcement. The results of this study have implications for the understanding of how reinforcement controls behavior. 相似文献
46.
Effects of differences between stimuli, responses, and reinforcer rates on conditional discrimination performance 下载免费PDF全文
In a discrete-trial conditional discrimination procedure, 4 pigeons obtained food reinforcers by pecking a key with a short latency on trials signaled by one stimulus and by pecking the same key with a long latency on trials signaled by a second stimulus. The physical difference between the two stimuli and the temporal separation between the latency values required for reinforcement were varied factorially over four sets of conditions, and the ratio of reinforcer rates for short and long latencies was varied within each set of conditions. Stimulus discrimination varied directly with both stimulus and response differences and was unaffected by the reinforcer ratio. Sensitivity to reinforcement, estimated by generalized-matching-law fits to the data within each set of conditions, varied directly with the response difference but inversely with the stimulus difference arranged between sets of conditions. Because variations in stimulus differences, response differences, and reinforcer differences did not have equivalent effects, these findings question the functional equivalence of the three terms of the discriminated operant: antecedent stimuli, behavior, and consequences. 相似文献
47.
48.
Claire Brechet 《The Journal of genetic psychology》2017,178(2):139-146
The author's purpose was to examine children's recognition of emotional facial expressions, by comparing two types of stimulus: photographs and drawings. The author aimed to investigate whether drawings could be considered as a more evocative material than photographs, as a function of age and emotion. Five- and 7-year-old children were presented with photographs and drawings displaying facial expressions of 4 basic emotions (i.e., happiness, sadness, anger, and fear) and were asked to perform a matching task by pointing to the face corresponding to the target emotion labeled by the experimenter. The photographs we used were selected from the Radboud Faces Database and the drawings were designed on the basis of both the facial components involved in the expression of these emotions and the graphic cues children tend to use when asked to depict these emotions in their own drawings. Our results show that drawings are better recognized than photographs, for sadness, anger, and fear (with no difference for happiness, due to a ceiling effect). And that the difference between the 2 types of stimuli tends to be more important for 5-year-olds compared to 7-year-olds. These results are discussed in view of their implications, both for future research and for practical application. 相似文献
49.
Dougher M Perkins DR Greenway D Koons A Chiasson C 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2002,78(1):63-93
The transformation of functions refers to the untrained acquisition of stimulus functions among members of stimulus equivalence classes or relational frames. Although it is widely assumed that contextual control over the transformation of fuctions must exist, this has not yet been conclusively demonstrated in laboratory studies. Four experiments are reported in which (a) stimulus equivalence classes were established, (b) a conditional stimulus function was trained for one member of each of the classes, and (c) multiple-exemplar procedures were used to train and test for contextual control over the transformation of the stimulus function within the classes and to assess whether it generalized to new equivalence classes. Although a significant amount of training was required, the procedures ultimately resulted in the contextual control of function transformation for 9 of 10 participants and generalized contextual control for 4 of 5 participants. 相似文献
50.
Kit Fine 《Journal of Philosophical Logic》2000,29(6):543-584
This paper provides a possible worlds semantics for the system of the author's previous paper The Logic of Essence. The basic idea behind the semantics is that a statement should be taken to be true in virtue of the nature of certain objects just in case it is true in any possible world compatible with the nature of those objects. It is shown that a slight variant of the original system is sound and complete under the proposed semantics. 相似文献