全文获取类型
收费全文 | 917篇 |
免费 | 252篇 |
国内免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
1179篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 16篇 |
2022年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 27篇 |
2020年 | 35篇 |
2019年 | 27篇 |
2018年 | 25篇 |
2017年 | 26篇 |
2016年 | 24篇 |
2015年 | 32篇 |
2014年 | 30篇 |
2013年 | 39篇 |
2012年 | 27篇 |
2011年 | 37篇 |
2010年 | 32篇 |
2009年 | 25篇 |
2008年 | 27篇 |
2007年 | 27篇 |
2006年 | 25篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 32篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 21篇 |
2000年 | 22篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 20篇 |
1995年 | 17篇 |
1994年 | 22篇 |
1993年 | 25篇 |
1992年 | 27篇 |
1991年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 30篇 |
1988年 | 22篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 23篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 23篇 |
1983年 | 22篇 |
1982年 | 25篇 |
1981年 | 24篇 |
1980年 | 22篇 |
1979年 | 22篇 |
1978年 | 31篇 |
1977年 | 32篇 |
1976年 | 25篇 |
1975年 | 12篇 |
排序方式: 共有1179条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
The current review summarizes the literature on competing stimulus assessments (CSAs). CSAs are pretreatment assessments designed to systematically identify stimuli that reduce problem behavior (PB), ostensibly through reinforcer competition or substitution. We report on the participant characteristics, outcomes, and predictive validity of published CSAs that included (a) no-stimulus control trial(s), (b) test trials during which each stimulus was available singly and noncontingently, and (c) measurement of PB and stimulus engagement or contact. Results showed that CSAs have broad utility across a variety of topographies and functions of PB. In the majority of CSA applications for which extended analyses, or validations, were performed, stimuli shown to reduce PB during the CSA produced similar reductions during extended analysis. This was the case regardless of topography or function of PB, or whether the stimuli were assumed to be “matched” to the stimulation thought to be produced by PB. Implications for future research are discussed. 相似文献
32.
Jonathan M. Hochmuth Brian J. Crowley-Koch Ron Van Houten 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2020,53(4):2053-2066
The gateway sign configuration has been effective at increasing motorist yielding and reducing speeds at crosswalks. A gateway configuration uses in-street signs at a crosswalk on each edge of the roadway and on each lane line. Although this intervention is effective at increasing motorist yielding at uncontrolled crosswalks, the limits of the intervention have yet to be tested. The present study examined if 1) the effects of the gateway intervention on one crosswalk would generalize to an untreated adjacent crosswalk, and 2) if the effects of an offset configuration of signs which partially treated each crosswalk could maximize the effects of that generalization. Experiment 1 showed that less yielding occurred at the untreated crosswalk than at the treated crosswalk, though yielding was higher than baseline. In Experiment 2, results showed that an offset gateway configuration could produce comparable levels of yielding at both crosswalks. 相似文献
33.
Cammarie Johnson Iver Iversen Paula Kenyon Per Holth Deisy G. de Souza 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2020,53(3):1290-1298
Murray Sidman's contributions to the science of behavior span many areas including avoidance behavior, coercion and its effects, stimulus control, errorless learning, programmed learning, stimulus equivalence, and single-subject methodology. He was also a great mentor to many and helped shape the discipline we now call behavior analysis. In this memoriam, we briefly highlight his scholarly legacy and share some personal anecdotes. 相似文献
34.
Daniel E. Conine Timothy R. Vollmer Hypatia A. Bolívar 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2020,53(2):744-766
Deficits in response to name (RTN) are an early indicator of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and RTN is a treatment goal in many early intervention curricula for children with ASD. However, little research has empirically evaluated methods for increasing RTN in children with ASD. We evaluated a series of conditions designed to increase RTN for 4 children with ASD using a multielement experimental design. The schedules of tangible reinforcement were thinned after mastery and generalization was tested across people and contexts. Tangible reinforcers were necessary to increase RTN for all 4 participants, and the schedule of reinforcement was successfully thinned with all participants after intervention. Generalization was also observed across people and experimental contexts. 相似文献
35.
Daniel R. Mitteer Kevin C. Luczynski Ciobha A. McKeown Victoria L. Cohrs 《Behavioral Interventions》2020,35(1):3-24
We evaluated whether teaching tacts of images with or without backgrounds would affect acquisition during teaching and generality across untaught targets with four children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Including backgrounds could slow acquisition but bolster generality across other media containing backgrounds, whereas removing backgrounds may improve acquisition but result in poorer generality. Overall, acquisition rates were similar across conditions for three children, although one child displayed consistently slower acquisition during the background condition. We rarely observed generality to untaught targets when programming extinction, but embedding differential reinforcement or teaching additional images with backgrounds led to equally successful outcomes irrespective of initial teaching conditions. Teaching with or without backgrounds may result in marginal differences in acquisition of teaching and untaught targets, especially when arranging supplemental procedures during tests for generality. We discuss the importance of intrasubject replication, considerations when testing for generality, and implications for practice. 相似文献
36.
Bilingual infants from 6‐ to 24‐months of age are more likely to generalize, flexibly reproducing actions on novel objects significantly more often than age‐matched monolingual infants are. In the current study, we examine whether the addition of novel verbal labels enhances memory generalization in a perceptually complex imitation task. We hypothesized that labels would provide an additional retrieval cue and aid memory generalization for bilingual infants. Specifically, we hypothesized that bilinguals might be more likely than monolinguals to map multiple perceptual features onto a novel label and therefore show enhanced generalization. Eighty‐seven 18‐month‐old monolingual and bilingual infants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions or a baseline control condition. In the experimental conditions, either no label or a novel label was added during demonstration and again at the beginning of the test session. After a 24‐hr delay, infants were tested with the same stimulus set to test cued recall and with a perceptually different but functionally equivalent stimulus set to test memory generalization. Bilinguals performed significantly above baseline on both cued recall and memory generalization in both experimental conditions, whereas monolinguals performed significantly above baseline only on cued recall in both experimental conditions. These findings show a difference between monolinguals and bilinguals in memory generalization and suggest that generalization differences between groups may arise from visual perceptual processing rather than linguistic processing. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/yXB4pM3fF2k 相似文献
37.
《心理科学》2025,48(3)
目前,研究关于儿童早期语言领域的统计学习能力是否随年龄发展尚未得到一致的结论,且缺乏在相同实验参数下对不同领域和感觉通道统计学习早期发展的系统考察。研究选择中班到二年级5~8岁的儿童,首次系统地考察了早期听觉和视觉通道中语言和非语言领域统计学习的发展。儿童首先在熟悉阶段听或看由无意义三联体构成的声音流或图片流;接着在测验阶段对三联体和非三联体做二择一判断。结果发现:1.儿童对听觉音节材料的统计学习能力从5~6岁快速发展,并在6岁时达到了和8岁儿童相当的水平,表明在儿童早期听觉语言领域的统计学习存在发展优势;2.从6岁起,儿童已能对视觉文字材料进行统计学习,且无论是视觉文字、物品图片还是听觉环境声音材料,其统计学习成绩均随年龄增长而提高。这些结果说明统计学习可能既遵循某种领域一般的原则,又受到输入刺激特性的限制,是一种多成分的能力。 相似文献
38.
Using a conditioned flavor aversion procedure with rats as subjects, the effect of the addition of a distractor stimulus on the magnitude of the latent inhibition effect was examined. Experiment 1 showed that latent inhibition to vinegar was attenuated by the addition of sucrose during preexposure. On the other hand, sucrose added during conditioning to vinegar did not attenuate latent inhibition. It was also found that the degree of latent inhibition to the vinegar-sucrose compound solution was less when vinegar alone was preexposed (i.e., when sucrose was added only during conditioning) than when the compound solution was preexposed (i.e., when sucrose was added both during preexposure and during conditioning). Experiment 2 gave similar results but with sucrose assigned as the target flavor and vinegar as the distractor. These findings are in full agreement with the generalization decrement account of latent inhibition. 相似文献
39.
Three autistic students were trained to request a specific object from an adult “supplier” with the sentence, “Give me —” and to deliver that object to another adult, the “director.” Subsequently, the degree to which the object offered by the supplier controlled the “Give me —” verbal response was assessed by delivering to the student an object other than the one requested. Despite knowing the names of all objects used in the experiment, students accepted and delivered to the director any object offered by the supplier regardless of its match with the requested object. After training to say “That's not it. Give me —” when nonrequested objects were offered, students responded differentially to requested and nonrequested objects, suggesting control of the “Give me —” response by the requested object, a characteristic of a mand. These results generalized across settings and objects. Results are discussed in terms of the training technique to establish manding and the functional analysis of the resulting verbal behavior. 相似文献
40.
Pigeons received variable-interval reinforcement for key pecking during presentations of horizontal and vertical line-orientation stimuli, while pecks during five intermediate orientations were extinguished. Lowest peck rates were observed during presentations of negative stimuli adjacent to the positive orientations while peck rate during 45 degrees (the intermediate negative orientation) was relatively high, i.e., there were negative contrast shoulders. When peck rates were manipulated in the positive orientations, peck rate in neithboring orientations changed in the opposite direction. Contrast shoulders faded after prolonged training. A second type of contrast, local contrast, was correlated with similarity of preceding stimulus and different average peck rates during different stages of the discrimination process. The data suggest that sequential local contrast accompanying the formation of a discrimination contributes to the form of generalization gradients. Blough's model of stimulus control predicts the changes in gradient form described here, but may not accurately depict the underlying process responsible for gradient form. 相似文献