首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   167篇
  免费   37篇
  国内免费   22篇
  2023年   1篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   10篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   12篇
  2018年   13篇
  2017年   17篇
  2016年   7篇
  2015年   16篇
  2014年   5篇
  2013年   34篇
  2012年   7篇
  2011年   7篇
  2010年   2篇
  2009年   6篇
  2008年   10篇
  2007年   7篇
  2006年   12篇
  2005年   4篇
  2004年   6篇
  2003年   2篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   2篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   1篇
  1997年   2篇
  1995年   1篇
  1992年   3篇
  1988年   1篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   2篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   3篇
  1981年   2篇
  1979年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1976年   1篇
排序方式: 共有226条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
The relation between tinnitus and short-term memory performance in varying background sounds is not well understood. In the present study a sample of 18 persons with tinnitus completed a serial recall test in three conditions, silence, masking and intermittent masking. The performance of a matched control group without tinnitus was also investigated. Based on the literature on the "irrelevant sound effect" we expected that the tinnitus group would perform worse during intermittent masking and that they would score lower overall compared to the control group. Results revealed no statistically significant differences between the groups, nor any group interaction within sound conditions for the serial recall test. Groups did however differ regarding subjective measures of concentration problems, anxiety and depression. Results are discussed in relation to thought suppression and distraction from tinnitus.  相似文献   
92.
不同刺激强度训练后一日龄小鸡的记忆形成   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
一日龄小鸡,分别接受不同浓度的回避性刺激物邻氨基苯甲酸甲酯(MeA)训练,间隔一定时间后测试记忆保持。主要结果为:5%和20%MeA训练,分别使动物记忆保持15分钟和50分钟左右,相当于记忆形成多阶段模型中的STM和ITM阶段。40%MeA训练,记忆保持到训练后8-10小时,相当于不依赖新糖蛋白合成的LTM形成的早期阶段。只有60%以上浓度的MeA训练才使记忆保持至少24小时。上述结果提示,利用弱化训练的方法,不仅可以将STM,ITM和LTM分离开,而且可把LTM形成的早期阶段分离出来独立研究,增加了对LTM新的理解,为进一步探讨记忆形成机制提供了一条新的途径。  相似文献   
93.
本研究采用双因素实验设计探讨了运动负荷强度和持续时间对 E-POC的影响。实验结果发现 ,运动过程中机体净耗氧量主要取决于运动持续时间 ,而运动后的 EPOC既受运动强度 ,也受持续时间的影响 ,但以前者的作用更加明显 ;运动后 VO2 的恢复速度快于血乳酸的消除 ,不同运动条件下的氧亏量变化主要取决于运动负荷的强度。  相似文献   
94.
In two studies, free-roaming male rats (aggressors) were shocked in the presence of male target rats restrained in either an upright or a supine posure. In addition, in Experiment II, two levels of aggressor shock intensity (0.8 mA or 2.0 mA) were used while targets received one of three levels of shock (0.5 mA, 1.5 mA, or 2.5 mA). In both studies, upright targets were attacked less than supine targets. Frequency of aggression was directly related to level of aggressor shock intensity in Experiment II. Also, attack by 0.8-mA aggressors against supine targets was inversely related to level of target shock intensity. The low level of attack against upright targets was interpreted in terms of a threat diaplay. Similarily, it was concluded that the target shock-intensity effect in Experiment II was due to specific threat behaviors displayed by those supine rats that received the highest-intensity shocks.  相似文献   
95.
Rats were trained on a free-operant avoidance procedure in which shock intensity was controlled by interresponse time. Shocks were random at a density of about 10 shocks per minute. Shock probability was response independent. As long as interresponse times remained less than the limit in effect, any shocks received were at the lower of two intensities (0.75 mA). Whenever interresponse times exceeded the limit, any shocks received were at the higher intensity (1.6 mA). The initial limit of 15 seconds was decreased in 3-second steps to either 6 or 3 seconds. All animals lever pressed to avoid higher intensity shock. As the interresponse time limit was reduced, the response rate during the lower intensity shock and the proportion of brief interresponse times increased. Substantial warmup effects were evident, particularly at the shorter interresponse-time limits. Shock intensity reduction without change in shock probability was effective in the acquisition and maintenance of avoidance responding, as well as in differentiation of interresponse times. This research suggests limitations on the generality of a safety signal interpretation of avoidance conditioning.  相似文献   
96.
Can the Simultaneous Experience of Opposing Emotions Really Occur?   总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0  
Various investigators have proposed that people may feel simultaneous positive and negative affect. However, experimental evidence from tests of a recent theory about the intensity of emotion (J. W. Brehm, 1999) suggests that even when they are invited by the experimental design, positive and negative emotions do not occur at the same time. When people have been instigated to feel a particular emotion, such as happiness, and then are given a reason (e.g., sad news) for not feeling happy, they report continued happiness but no increase in sadness unless the reason for feeling sad is very great, in which case sadness replaces happiness. The present paper briefly reviews the underlying theory and evidence, and discusses implications.
Jack W. BrehmEmail:
  相似文献   
97.
Two experiments investigated the roles of shock intensity and scheduling in selective punishment of interresponse times. In each experiment the punishment contingencies were imposed on a background of rats' responding maintained by a variable-interval schedule of food presentation. In Experiment 1 all interresponse times greater than 8 seconds produced shock. In Experiment 2 all interresponse times greater than 8 seconds but less than 12 seconds produced shock. In each experiment shock intensity was initially 0.3 milliamperes (mA) and then was varied through an ascending sequence ranging from 0.1 mA to 0.4 mA, in 0.1-mA increments. Experiment 1 produced response-rate increases at low intensities (0.1 and 0.2 mA) but eliminated responding at the remaining intensities. Experiment 2 produced response-rate increases only with 0.1-mA shock, although responding was maintained at all shock parameters investigated. Analysis of the interresponse times per opportunity showed differential suppression of the targeted responses in all cases except the high-intensity shock phases of Experiment 1. The current data support and extend previous studies of selective interresponse-time-dependent shock schedules but suggest that response-rate increases are not a necessary outcome of this type of procedure. The view that variable-interval schedules of shock presentation selectively target long interresponse times was also supported.  相似文献   
98.
99.
Although response‐dependent shock often suppresses responding, response facilitation can occur. In two experiments, we examined the suppressive and facilitative effects of shock by manipulating shock intensity and the interresponse times that produced shock. Rats' lever presses were reinforced on a variable‐interval 40‐s schedule of food presentation. Shock followed either long or short interresponse times. Shock intensity was raised from 0.05 mA to 0.4 mA or 0.8 mA. Overall, shock contingent on long interresponse times punished long interresponse times and increased response rates. Shock contingent on short interresponse times punished short interresponse times and decreased response rates. In Experiment 1, raising the range of interresponse times that produced shock enhanced these effects. In Experiment 2, the effects of shock intensity depended on the interresponse times that produced shock. When long interresponse times produced shock, low intensities increased response rates. High intensities decreased response rates. When short interresponse times produced shock, high shock intensities punished short interresponse times and decreased response rates more than low intensities. The results may explain why punishment procedures occasionally facilitate responding and establish parameters for future studies of punishment.  相似文献   
100.
Drawing on quasi‐experimental data from participants in structured and moderated deliberations on sexual minority rights in Poland, this study tests the relationship between attitude importance, certainty, intensity, and extremity. This study also examines the antecedents of these attributes and their consequences for several democratically important outcomes (i.e., perceived disagreement, negative affect, attitude polarization, self‐reported polarization, argument repertoire, and political participation). This study further asks whether these attributes exert different effects than a composite attitude strength index. Although factor analysis suggests that these attributes represent one construct, they are differently affected by deliberation and exert differential effects. The analyses further reveal that relying on the composite index obscures substantial information and inaccurately represents strength‐related processes and functions. Theoretical, methodological, and practical implications are discussed.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号