全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4081篇 |
免费 | 570篇 |
国内免费 | 160篇 |
专业分类
4811篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 33篇 |
2024年 | 59篇 |
2023年 | 96篇 |
2022年 | 135篇 |
2021年 | 149篇 |
2020年 | 250篇 |
2019年 | 182篇 |
2018年 | 178篇 |
2017年 | 168篇 |
2016年 | 154篇 |
2015年 | 151篇 |
2014年 | 149篇 |
2013年 | 402篇 |
2012年 | 147篇 |
2011年 | 183篇 |
2010年 | 110篇 |
2009年 | 166篇 |
2008年 | 152篇 |
2007年 | 164篇 |
2006年 | 167篇 |
2005年 | 129篇 |
2004年 | 120篇 |
2003年 | 112篇 |
2002年 | 94篇 |
2001年 | 71篇 |
2000年 | 78篇 |
1999年 | 66篇 |
1998年 | 56篇 |
1997年 | 70篇 |
1996年 | 55篇 |
1995年 | 54篇 |
1994年 | 67篇 |
1993年 | 46篇 |
1992年 | 44篇 |
1991年 | 36篇 |
1990年 | 29篇 |
1989年 | 21篇 |
1987年 | 35篇 |
1986年 | 31篇 |
1985年 | 37篇 |
1984年 | 38篇 |
1983年 | 33篇 |
1982年 | 34篇 |
1981年 | 32篇 |
1980年 | 28篇 |
1979年 | 32篇 |
1978年 | 28篇 |
1977年 | 51篇 |
1976年 | 42篇 |
1975年 | 26篇 |
排序方式: 共有4811条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
We assessed the academic performance of a 14-year-old boy with insect phobia in the context of feared stimuli. The dependent measure was math calculation rate across three conditions that varied therapist statements about the presence of crickets and the actual presence of live crickets. Subsequent treatment consisted of graduated exposure and contingent rewards for math problem completion. Assessment results indicated that the boy's performance was consistently low in the presence of live crickets but not when he was spuriously informed that crickets were present (the primary referral concern). Treatment results indicated no effect from exposure alone and a dramatic effect when exposure was combined with contingent rewards. 相似文献
182.
Results from a number of studies have shown an inverse relationship between stereotypic behavior and object manipulation. The purposes of this study were to determine whether techniques similar to those used previously (prompting and reinforcement) would be effective in increasing object manipulation under both prompted and unprompted conditions, and to ascertain whether increases in object manipulation would result in decreases in stereotypic self-injurious behavior (SIB). Two individuals with developmental disabilities who engaged in SIB maintained by automatic reinforcement participated. Results showed that object manipulation increased from baseline levels when experimenters prompted participants to manipulate leisure items, but that object manipulation was not maintained under unprompted conditions, and rates of SIB stayed within baseline levels. We then attempted to increase object manipulation further by (a) reinforcing object manipulation, (b) blocking SIB while reinforcing manipulation, and (c) preventing SIB by applying protective equipment while reinforcing object manipulation. Reinforcing object manipulation alone did not affect levels of object manipulation. Blocking effectively reduced attempts to engage in SIB for 1 participant but produced no increase in object manipulation. When the 2nd participant was prevented from engaging in SIB through the use of protective equipment, rates of object manipulation increased dramatically but were not maintained when the equipment was removed. These results suggest that stimulation derived from object manipulation, even when supplemented with arbitrary reinforcement, may not compete with stimulation produced by stereotypic SIB; therefore, direct interventions to reduce SIB are required. 相似文献
183.
Rolf Loeber Ph.D. Stephanie M. Green M.A. Benjamin B. Lahey Ph.D. Mary Anne G. Christ M.A. Paul J. Frick Ph.D. 《Journal of child and family studies》1992,1(1):21-41
Retrospective and prospective reports of the onset of disruptive child behaviors were analyzed in a sample of clinic-referred boys. The younger boys (ages 7–9 years), compared with the older boys (ages 10–12 years), showed the highest level of disruptive behavior and, judging from mothers' reports, had the fastest progression of onsets from less serious to more serious problem behaviors. Despite some overlap, developmental sequences in problem behavior within the domains of hyperactivity/inattention, oppositional behavior, and conduct problems were similar across the two age groups. This was also the case for developmental sequences of overt or confrontive problem behaviors and covert or concealing conduct problems. Sequences of the onset of oppositional behavior and conduct problems were validated through prospective data, based on the information from mothers, teachers, and boys themselves. The implications of the findings are discussed for the formulation of developmental pathways of behavior and the analysis of causal factors. 相似文献
184.
Religiousness, Health, and Health Behavior in Public-Use Data of the National Center for Health Statistics 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Studies of religiousness and health-related variables in large, population-based cross-sectional or, preferably, longitudinal
studies, which are often prohibitively expensive, are important to complement findings from the more commonly performed studies.
Inadequately known among social science researchers, the national health surveys of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s
National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS) offer large, high-quality data sets to the public at no or nominal cost and hence
offer important opportunities for research in the area of religion and health, religion and reproductive behavior, sociology
of religion and psychology of religion. This report provides an overview of the data sets and a bibliography of prior research
using these data, which is intended to suggest how the data of NCHS may be further exploited by researchers of religiousness
and health.
相似文献
R. F. GillumEmail: |
185.
Joyce Vogel Susan Phillips Keane Judith C. Conger 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1988,10(1):49-64
The present study was designed to assess whether the conversational behavior of children separated into accepted, rejected, and mixed-status dyads differed along the dimensions of listening to, paying attention to, and indicating interest in their dyadic partner. Conversations of 40 third-grade dyads were analyzed as they occurred in a 2- min waiting period and a 4- min conversation period. Seventeen categories of verbal behaviors that indexed these dimensions were used as content codes. Multivariate analyses of variance performed on the observational data yielded significant main effects for status, sex, and experimental condition. Results are interpreted in terms of previous social skills research, and suggestions for future research are offered. 相似文献
186.
Three autistic students were trained to request a specific object from an adult “supplier” with the sentence, “Give me —” and to deliver that object to another adult, the “director.” Subsequently, the degree to which the object offered by the supplier controlled the “Give me —” verbal response was assessed by delivering to the student an object other than the one requested. Despite knowing the names of all objects used in the experiment, students accepted and delivered to the director any object offered by the supplier regardless of its match with the requested object. After training to say “That's not it. Give me —” when nonrequested objects were offered, students responded differentially to requested and nonrequested objects, suggesting control of the “Give me —” response by the requested object, a characteristic of a mand. These results generalized across settings and objects. Results are discussed in terms of the training technique to establish manding and the functional analysis of the resulting verbal behavior. 相似文献
187.
Brown AM Deacon BJ Abramowitz JS Dammann J Whiteside SP 《Behaviour research and therapy》2007,45(4):819-828
Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) and pharmacotherapy are the most well-established treatments for childhood anxiety disorders. This study examined how parents (N=71) seeking treatment for their child's anxiety disorder perceive the acceptability, believability, and effectiveness of these treatments. While both treatments were perceived favorably, CBT was rated as more acceptable, believable, and effective in the short- and long-term. Children's treatment history influenced parents' perceptions of pharmacotherapy, with parents of children with no treatment history perceiving medication treatment as less acceptable and believable than parents of children with a history of medication alone or in combination with behavior therapy. No effect of treatment history was found for perceptions of CBT. Significant positive correlations emerged between parents' perceived acceptance and believability for pharmacotherapy and child age and level of dysfunction due to their child's anxiety, respectively. The level of the child's anxiety was not significantly correlated with parents' perceptions of either CBT or pharmacotherapy. Our results suggest that parents of anxious children prefer CBT to medication for the treatment of their child's anxiety disorder. Directions for future research are discussed. 相似文献
188.
Polman H Orobio de Castro B Koops W van Boxtel HW Merk WW 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2007,35(4):522-535
The present meta-analytic review aimed to clarify divergent findings concerning the relation between reactive and proactive aggression in children and adolescents. Fifty-one studies with 17,965 participants were included in the analysis. A significant correlation between reactive and proactive aggression was found. The strength of this relation varied considerably between studies, from -.10 to .89. Observational assessment and tilt/noise tasks were associated with smaller correlations than questionnaires. Within the large group of questionnaire studies, studies disentangling the form and function of aggression found lower correlations than studies that did not disentangle form and function. Among questionnaire studies, higher reliability was associated with larger correlations. Effect size did not depend on other study characteristics such as sample type, age, and informant type. It is concluded that reactive and proactive aggression are most clearly distinguished with behavioral observations and questionnaires that unravel form and functions of aggression. 相似文献
189.
This paper studies mixed-gender group interactions in a strategic game where group members are sequentially eliminated till
a single winner takes all. Study 1 tests the hypothesis that female contestants are retained till final rounds where they
are eliminated. Using observational data from the US television show The Weakest Link (20 shows), results show that females are finalists but not winners. In a laboratory study (Study 2, 67 Berkeley undergraduates),
we show that this effect is attenuated when winnings are shared among finalists (versus one winner takes all) due to the reduction
in competitive pressures in the context.
This research was supported in part by the Junior Faculty Research Grant awarded by the University of California at Berkeley
to the second author. 相似文献
190.
The looking behavior of children with pervasive developmental disorder (PDD) and age- and IQ-matched normal control children was studied using infrared oculography. Stimuli varying in complexity and topic were presented to test whether children with PDD have specific abnormalities in looking behavior to complex stimuli and/or to faces. All children showed more and longer fixations on the complex objects than on the simple objects, especially the complex nonsense figure, but group differences were not found. The results show no evidence for specific abnormalities in looking behavior to either faces or to complex stimuli in high functioning children with PDD. 相似文献