全文获取类型
收费全文 | 237篇 |
免费 | 47篇 |
国内免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
291篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 2篇 |
2024年 | 2篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 9篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 12篇 |
2015年 | 9篇 |
2014年 | 14篇 |
2013年 | 46篇 |
2012年 | 13篇 |
2011年 | 6篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 5篇 |
2007年 | 15篇 |
2006年 | 9篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有291条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
32.
Studies indicate that there is a positive relationship between openness to experience and creativity. However, relatively little attention has been given to the mechanism of this relationship. On the basis of previous findings, we hypothesized a conceptual model and tested the mediating role of intrinsic motivation and the creative process engagement in the relationship between openness to experience and creativity. One hundred and ninety‐eight undergraduates in Malaysia participated in the study and completed an online measure of openness to new experience, creativity, intrinsic motivation, and the creative process engagement. Consistent with the hypothesis, people who scored high on openness reported high intrinsic motivation. The high motivation enhanced engagement in creativity‐related activities, which in turn, improved self‐rated creativity. The findings not only shed light on mechanisms that underlie in the openness‐creativity linkage but they also highlight the importance of intrinsic motivation and creative process engagement in the linkage. Together, the study extends the effect of personality trait on creativity and offers a new direction for future studies. 相似文献
33.
34.
35.
Schuck NW Gaschler R Frensch PA 《Advances in cognitive psychology / University of Finance and Management in Warsaw》2012,8(2):83-97
Much research has been conducted aimed at the representations and mechanisms that enable learning of sequential structures. A central debate concerns the question whether item-item associations (i.e., in the sequence A-B-C-D, B comes after A) or associations of item and serial list position (i.e., B is the second item in the list) are used to represent serial order. Previously, we showed that in a variant of the implicit serial reaction time task, the sequence representation contains associations between serial position and item information (Schuck, Gaschler, Keisler, & Frensch, 2011). Here, we applied models and research methods from working memory research to implicit serial learning to replicate and extend our findings. The experiment involved three sessions of sequence learning. Results support the view that participants acquire knowledge about order structure (item-item associations) and about ordinal structure (serial position-item associations). Analyses suggest that only the simultaneous use of the two types of knowledge acquisition can explain learning-related performance increases. Additionally, our results indicate that serial list position information plays a role very early in learning and that inter-item associations increasingly control behavior in later stages. 相似文献
36.
Shih SI 《Cognitive psychology》2008,56(3):210-236
An attention cascade model is proposed to account for attentional blinks in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) of stimuli. Data were collected using single characters in a single RSVP stream at 10Hz [Shih, S., & Reeves, A. (2007). Attentional capture in rapid serial visual presentation. Spatial Vision, 20(4), 301-315], and single words, in both single and dual RSVP streams at 19Hz [Potter, M. C., Staub, A., & O'Connor, D. H. (2002). The time course of competition for attention: Attention is initially labile. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(5), 1149-1162]. The model adopts similar architecture of the cognitive accounts of attentional blinks and employs computational details from theories of attention gating. The model has elaborated working memory and attention control mechanism. Both bottom-up and top-down salience are explicit in the model. Quantitative fits are good and the model parameters have plausible values. The model handles stimulus competition, lag 1 sparing, intrusion errors, and magnitude of the dip; it also accounts for commonly observed effects such as stimulus similarities (local and global), target+1 blank, and stimulus salience. 相似文献
37.
Tim Curran 《Trends in cognitive sciences》2001,5(12):109
Complex information, such as that required for motor skills, can be learned implicitly, without awareness. Much debate has centered on the appropriate methods for proving that implicit learning is not influenced by explicit awareness. A recent study by Destrebecqz and Cleeremans has provided compelling evidence for implicit sequence learning without awareness by using the ‘method of opposition’. 相似文献
38.
采用RSVP范式验证了两个序列呈现的刺激可以被并行加工的假设。实验一发现:在两个目标刺激(Target和Probe,以下简称T和P)间无干扰刺激时,T的反应时随着T、P之间的间隔显著减小,其模式符合并行加工模型的预测。而实验二发现:当T和P之间有干扰刺激(即T后呈现后续掩蔽)时,T的反应时并不随着T、P的间隔而减小,其模式符合序列加工模型的预测。由此可推论,只要两个序列呈现的刺激之间没有干扰刺激,可以对他们进行并行的注意加工,但是会受到两个刺激之间的时间间隔的影响。 相似文献
39.
以往研究发现个体能够同时巩固一定数量的项目信息进入视觉工作记忆系统。本研究将考察视觉工作记忆的巩固容量是否会受到分配到记忆项目上的注意资源量的影响。实验1在同时序列掩蔽范式的基础上, 用控制记忆项目呈现区域的大小的方法操纵被试的注意范围, 从而达到操纵分配到单个项目上的注意资源量的目的。研究结果显示, 被试的记忆成绩随着注意范围的扩大显著降低, 但在并行和序列呈现条件之间不存在显著差异; 实验2则证实实验1中对注意资源量分配的控制不会影响对项目的知觉编码效率。本研究结果表明, 分配在记忆项目上的注意资源量能够影响记忆巩固的效率, 但不会影响巩固容量。 相似文献
40.