全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8888篇 |
免费 | 783篇 |
国内免费 | 293篇 |
专业分类
9964篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 22篇 |
2023年 | 117篇 |
2022年 | 75篇 |
2021年 | 122篇 |
2020年 | 218篇 |
2019年 | 237篇 |
2018年 | 207篇 |
2017年 | 223篇 |
2016年 | 201篇 |
2015年 | 128篇 |
2014年 | 150篇 |
2013年 | 544篇 |
2012年 | 130篇 |
2011年 | 129篇 |
2010年 | 115篇 |
2009年 | 146篇 |
2008年 | 140篇 |
2007年 | 147篇 |
2006年 | 147篇 |
2005年 | 124篇 |
2004年 | 106篇 |
2003年 | 97篇 |
2002年 | 86篇 |
2001年 | 68篇 |
2000年 | 60篇 |
1999年 | 70篇 |
1998年 | 54篇 |
1997年 | 47篇 |
1996年 | 64篇 |
1995年 | 45篇 |
1994年 | 43篇 |
1993年 | 42篇 |
1992年 | 40篇 |
1991年 | 34篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 35篇 |
1987年 | 28篇 |
1985年 | 465篇 |
1984年 | 530篇 |
1983年 | 438篇 |
1982年 | 542篇 |
1981年 | 511篇 |
1980年 | 533篇 |
1979年 | 479篇 |
1978年 | 507篇 |
1977年 | 387篇 |
1976年 | 390篇 |
1975年 | 316篇 |
1974年 | 308篇 |
1973年 | 254篇 |
排序方式: 共有9964条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Ann R. Beck 《Brain and language》1984,22(2):320-338
The ability of aphasic subjects to process the sounds and meanings of ongoing speech was tested. Subjects, 10 aphasic and 20 control, heard test sentences which contained one member of phonemically similar word pairs in one of three semantic contexts: congruent, neutral, and noncongruent. Immediately after hearing a sentence, subjects were to indicate which member of the word pair had been in the sentence. All subject groups had similar overall patterns of response to the different semantic contexts. The division of aphasic subjects into groups of high and low comprehenders revealed response differences. High comprehending aphasic subjects, like control subjects, demonstrated interactive processing of the sounds and meaning of speech while low comprehending aphasic subjects did not. 相似文献
72.
Robert S. Pierce 《Brain and language》1984,22(2):339-349
This study assessed 13 aphasic subjects' knowledge of the multiple meanings of homographs. The results indicated that the various meanings were less available to aphasic subjects than to normal subjects. In addition, specific meanings became less available as they became less typical although the number of meanings associated with a homograph did not influence performance. Aphasia type also did not influence performance. Performance on the experimental test was significantly correlated with auditory comprehension level and picture naming ability. The results were related to recent findings on semantic organization in aphasia. 相似文献
73.
Comparing visual field asymmetries for bilaterally presented words and corresponding line drawings, we found an RVF advantage for words and no visual field asymmetry for line drawings. We suggested that the RVF advantage previously obtained by Young, Bion, and Ellis (Brain and Language, 11, 54–65, 1980) for bilaterally presented line drawings may have resulted from a forced order of report procedure, noting that the RVF advantage was greater on trials on which subjects were forced to report in the nonpreferred right-to-left order. Young and Ellis (Brain and Language, 20, 166–171, 1983) attempt to discredit this claim and maintain their hypothesis that the RVF advantage for line drawings is attributable to better temporary storage of these stimuli by the left than the right hemisphere. In the present article, we present in greater detail our arguments for the effects of forced order of report on the perception of bilaterally presented stimuli and refute Young and Ellis's ibid. criticisms of this proposal. 相似文献
74.
The nature of the phonological disorder in conduction aphasia 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
S E Kohn 《Brain and language》1984,23(1):97-115
Sequences of attempts to name pictured objects were used to examine phonological dysfunction in three diagnostic subgroups of aphasia. A prevalence of "phonologically-oriented sequences" (i.e., those sequences that contained only attempts with a phonological resemblance to the target word) was found to be a diagnostic criterion for conduction aphasia. When compared to a group of Broca's (n = 7) and Wernicke's (n = 5) aphasics, all the conduction aphasics (n = 6) produced proportionately more of such phonologically-oriented sequences on a picture naming test. An examination of the phonologically-oriented sequences produced by the conduction aphasics indicates that speech production in conduction aphasia involves dysfunction at an early stage of sound-encoding. The theoretical implications of this view are discussed. 相似文献
75.
The major theories and research approaches as to the etiology and development of stuttering are reviewed from a historical perspective. Findings are drawn from different sources indicating that language deficits are an initial contributing factor and a continuing component of this disorder. Those subsets of children which constitute high-risk groups are identified. It is suggested that researchers begin investigating the underlying organic bases of stuttering and its relationship to stages of language development. Finally, clinicians are encouraged to employ a therapy program that is concerned with all areas of language, rather than emphasizing treating only the symptoms of stuttering behavior. 相似文献
76.
Michele Andrisin Wittig 《Intelligence》1984,8(4):305-313
Three versions of Piaget's water-level-horizontality task (multiple choice, drawing, and apparatus tests) were administered to 121 female and 74 male adult volunteers. Sex differences and container orientation effects were replicated. Men were more accurate than women and horizontal and vertical container orientations evoked more accurate responses than oblique orientation. Reliability estimates ranged from .78 to .97 and correlations among the measures ranged from .78 to .88. The relative merits of alternative water-level testing and scoring methods and the orientation and type of stimuli to be used are discussed. 相似文献
77.
William S. Yovetich 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》1984,9(1):11-20
Message therapy is a clinical procedure designed to be used as a carry-over technique with school-age stutterers or as a sole technique with the preschooler. The focus of therapy is on a unit of communication rather than on the moment or place of stuttering. One of the basic assumptions of the procedure is that the child is beginning to monitor how he/she is speaking rather than what is being communicated (i.e., the “message”)—each task requiring a different cognitive process. The emphasis of the therapy is to redirect the child's attention to “what he/she is saying.” The clinical procedures are discussed as they relate to the specific population. 相似文献
78.
Marcel E. Wingate 《Journal of Fluency Disorders》1984,9(1):21-29
The rationale for and method of computing a measure of stuttering recurrence that is superior to methods previously developed is presented. This measure, the recurrence ratio, contrasts especially with the traditional measure of “consistency,” the percentage measure, which yields misleading values. 相似文献
79.
Jan-Eric Gustafsson 《Intelligence》1984,8(3):179-203
Models of the structure of cognitive abilities suggested by Spearman, Thurstone, Guilford, Vernon and Cattell-Horn are reviewed. It is noted that some of the models include a general intellectual factor (g) while others do not. It is also noted that some models are nonhierarchical, while in others more narrow abilities are subsumed under broader abilities in a hierarchical pattern. An empirical study in which a test battery of 16 tests was administered to some 1000 subjects in the 6th grade is reported. Using the LISREL technique to test different models, good support is obtained for oblique primary factors in the Thurstone tradition as well as for the second-order factors fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and general visualization hypothesized by Cattell and Horn. It is also found, however, that the second-order factor of fluid intelligence i is identical with a third-order g-factor. On the basis of these results a three-level model (the HILI-model) is suggested, with the g-factor at the top, two broad factors reflecting the ability to deal with verbal and figural information, respectively, at the second-order level, and the primary factors in the Thurstone and Guilford tradition at the lowest level. It is argued that most previously suggested models are special cases of the HILI-model. 相似文献
80.
The present study explored individual differences in performance of a geometric analogies task. Whereas past studies employed true/false or two-alternative items, the present research included four-alternative items and studied eye movements and confidence judgements for each item performance as well as latency and error. Item difficulty proved to be a function of an interaction between the number of response alternatives and the number of elements in items, especially for subjects lower in fluid-analytic reasoning ability. Results were interpreted using two hypothesized performance strategies: constructive matching and response elimination. The less efficient of these, response elimination, seemed to be used more by lower ability subjects on more difficult items. While two previous theories resemble one or the other of these strategies, neither alone seems to capture the complexity of adaptive problem solving. It appears that a comprehensive theory should incorporate strategy shifting as a function of item difficulty and subject ability.Componential models, based in part on past research, revealed that a justification component was activated and deactivated depending upon the nature of the analogy being solved. In addition, two new components, spatial inference and spatial application, were identified as important on some items, suggesting that different geometric analogy items invoke different cognitive processing components. Thus, a comprehensive theory should also describe component activation and deactivation. 相似文献