全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1662篇 |
免费 | 535篇 |
国内免费 | 197篇 |
专业分类
2394篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 48篇 |
2022年 | 33篇 |
2021年 | 59篇 |
2020年 | 108篇 |
2019年 | 109篇 |
2018年 | 85篇 |
2017年 | 103篇 |
2016年 | 88篇 |
2015年 | 62篇 |
2014年 | 92篇 |
2013年 | 194篇 |
2012年 | 77篇 |
2011年 | 79篇 |
2010年 | 55篇 |
2009年 | 70篇 |
2008年 | 66篇 |
2007年 | 65篇 |
2006年 | 60篇 |
2005年 | 55篇 |
2004年 | 63篇 |
2003年 | 49篇 |
2002年 | 47篇 |
2001年 | 43篇 |
2000年 | 40篇 |
1999年 | 37篇 |
1998年 | 43篇 |
1997年 | 33篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 36篇 |
1994年 | 37篇 |
1993年 | 32篇 |
1992年 | 22篇 |
1991年 | 29篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1989年 | 26篇 |
1988年 | 22篇 |
1987年 | 24篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 23篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 23篇 |
1982年 | 19篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 20篇 |
1979年 | 22篇 |
1978年 | 20篇 |
1977年 | 20篇 |
1976年 | 31篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有2394条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
A comparison of forward and backward procedures for the acquisition of response chains in humans 下载免费PDF全文
Weiss KM 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1978,29(2):255-259
Ten university students each learned four separate six-link response chains, two forward and two backward. All 10 subjects made fewer errors in the forward procedure. It was concluded that the forward procedure is superior because each link of the response chain is acquired by direct reinforcement. 相似文献
992.
Response rate and changeover performance on concurrent variable-interval schedules 总被引:6,自引:6,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Six pigeons were exposed to variable-interval schedules arranged on one, two, three, and four response keys. The reinforcement rate was also varied across conditions. Numbers of responses, the time spent responding, the number of reinforcements, and the number of changeovers between keys were recorded. Response rates on each key were an increasing function of reinforcement rate on that key and a decreasing function of the reinforcement rate on other keys. Response and time-allocation ratios under-matched ratios of obtained reinforcements. Three sets of equations were developed to express changeover rate as a function of response rate, time allocation, and reinforcement rate respectively. These functions were then applied to a broad range of experiments in the literature in order to test their generality. Further expressions were developed to account for changeover rates reported in experiments where changeover delays were varied. 相似文献
993.
Effect of variable-interval punishment on the behavior of humans in variable-interval schedules of monetary reinforcement 下载免费PDF全文
One male and three female human subjects pressed a button for monetary reinforcement under a range of variable-interval schedules specifying different frequencies of reinforcement. On alternate days, responding was also punished (by subtraction of money) according to a variable-interval 170-second schedule. In the absence of punishment, the rate of responding was an increasing negatively accelerated function of reinforcement frequency, as predicted by Herrnstein's equation. The effect of the punishment schedule was to suppress responding under lower frequencies of reinforcement; responding under higher reinforcement frequencies was much less affected. This was reflected in an increase in the value of KH (the constant expressing the reinforcement frequency corresponding to the half-maximal response rate), whereas there was no significant change in the value of Rmax (the constant expressing the maximum response rate). Previous results had shown that variable-ratio punishment resulted in a change in the values of both constants (Bradshaw, Szabadi, and Bevan, 1977). The results of the present study were consistent with the concept that the suppressive effects of punishment on responding depend on the nature of the punishment schedule. 相似文献
994.
Short-term memory in the rhesus monkey: A behavioral analysis of delayed-response performance 下载免费PDF全文
Kojima S 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1980,33(3):359-368
This study obtained quantitative data on the bodily orientations of rhesus monkeys in a delayed-response task and determined whether such orientations mediate the correct response in a choice trial. The basic task was a two-key chain schedule with the key leading to food signaled in the initial component. During the subsequent delay interval, the signal was removed, but it was necessary that one of the keys be pressed to advance the schedule to the terminal choice component. The position of the key pressed thus indicated orientation during the delay interval. When the monkeys had free access to the left and right keys, they tended to press the key leading to food throughout the chain schedule components and received food on more than 85% of the trials, even when the delay was extended to 20 seconds. However, when orientation toward the food key was disrupted by forcing the monkeys to press an extraneous center key during the delay, choice performance deteriorated. Requiring the center key presses early, rather than late, in the delay component had a strong disruptive effect. The relation of the results to the mediating coding-response hypothesis is discussed. 相似文献
995.
Learning by “following”, probably a common means by which behaviors are socially transmitted from adults to young in many species, was analyzed. Pigeons first learned to eat from a human hand. When the hand then approached an operant key and pecked it, the pigeons followed and quickly learned to do the same, thereby demonstrating social learning. When the hand only led the birds to the area of the key, without demonstrating the key-peck response, the birds learned as rapidly as with a key-peck demonstration. Birds also learned, but less reliably and more slowly, when they could observe the hand's responses but were constrained and unable to follow. “Following” was also shown to engender very rapid learning of a more complex, two-member response chain. 相似文献
996.
Three female human subjects pressed a button for monetary reinforcement in a range of variable-interval schedules specifying different frequencies of reinforcement. On alternate days, responding was also punished (by subtracting money) according to a variable-ratio 34 schedule. In the absence of punishment, rate of responding was an increasing negatively accelerated function of reinforcement frequency; the relationship between response rate and reinforcement frequency conformed to Herrnstein's equation. The effect of the punishment schedule was to suppress responding at all frequencies of reinforcement. This was reflected in a change in the values of both constants in Herrnstein's equation: the value of the theoretical maximum response-rate parameter was reduced, while the parameter describing the reinforcement frequency corresponding to the half-maximal response rate was increased. 相似文献
997.
Echolalia, the parroting of the speech of others, is a severe communication disorder frequently associated with childhood schizophrenia and mental retardation. Two echolalic children, one schizophrenic and one retarded, were treated in a multiple-baseline design across subjects. Each child was taught to make an appropriate, nonecholalic verbal response (i.e., “I don't know”) to a small set of previously echoed questions. After such training, this response generalized across a broad set of untrained questions that had formerly been echoed. The results obtained were the same irrespective of the specific experimenter who presented the questions. Further, each child discriminated appropriately between those questions that had previously been echoed and those that had not. Followup probes showed that treatment gains were maintained one month later. The procedure is economical, in that it produces a rapid and widespread cessation of echolalic responding. 相似文献
998.
It has been hypothesized that the magnitude-of-reinforcement effect may differ in closed and open experimental economies. We determined the relationship between magnitude of reinforcement and response rate in three feeding conditions: a closed economy in which total intake was unrestricted, a closed economy in which total intake was restricted so as to maintain body weight at 85% of free-feeding weight, and a traditional open economy in which subjects received food outside the experimental session. In the closed economies, regardless of body weight, the rats responded faster for smaller pellets and when the fixed ratio for pellets was higher. In the open economy, there was no reliable effect of pellet size or pellet cost on response rate. It is concluded that although there are circumstances in which response rate is an immediate function of the parameters of reinforcement, rate is not necessarily a measure of response strength. Response rate may instead, or additionally, contribute to a strategy of reducing the costs associated with resource utilization. 相似文献
999.
The area between two item characteristic curves 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Nambury S. Raju 《Psychometrika》1988,53(4):495-502
Formulas for computing the exact signed and unsigned areas between two item characteristic curves (ICCs) are presented. It is further shown that when thec parameters are unequal, the area between two ICCs is infinite. The significance of the exact area measures for item bias research is discussed.The author expresses his appreciation to Jeffrey A. Slinde, Stephen Steinhaus, Audrey Qualls-Payne, Ivo Molenaar, and two anonymous reviewers for their very helpful and constructive comments. 相似文献
1000.
Concurrent phencyclidine and saccharin access: presentation of an alternative reinforcer reduces drug intake. 总被引:2,自引:3,他引:2 下载免费PDF全文
M E Carroll 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1985,43(1):131-144
Six monkeys self-administered orally delivered phencyclidine ("angel dust") and saccharin under concurrent fixed-ratio 16 schedules during daily three-hour sessions. Liquid deliveries were contingent upon lip-contact responses on solenoid-operated drinking spouts. Three saccharin concentrations (0.003%, 0.03% and 0.3%, wt/vol) were tested in a nonsystematic order. For each saccharin concentration, the following series of phencyclidine concentrations (mg/ml) was presented: 0.25, 0.5, 1, 0.25 (retest), 0.125, 0.0625, 0.0312, 0.25 (retest) and 0 (water with stimuli signaling phencyclidine). As the saccharin concentration increased, the number of drug deliveries decreased, and the peaks of the concentration-response functions were shifted to the right. The lowest saccharin concentration (0.003%, wt/vol) maintained responding in excess of phencyclidine levels in only one monkey. The two higher saccharin concentrations maintained behavior far in excess of phencyclidine, but saccharin deliveries decreased in some monkeys as phencyclidine concentration and intake (mg/kg) increased. The time course and patterns of phencyclidine-reinforced responding were also altered when saccharin was concurrently available. The results are discussed in terms of strategies to reduce drug-reinforced behavior, preference between different reinforcers, and measures of reinforcing efficacy. 相似文献