全文获取类型
收费全文 | 285篇 |
免费 | 52篇 |
国内免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
347篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 2篇 |
2024年 | 12篇 |
2023年 | 7篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 13篇 |
2016年 | 18篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 7篇 |
2013年 | 27篇 |
2012年 | 12篇 |
2011年 | 12篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 9篇 |
2008年 | 10篇 |
2007年 | 17篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 19篇 |
2003年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有347条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
221.
胡林英 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2008,29(1):20-22
在对1999年以来我国脑死亡问题的研究成果进行初步梳理的基础上,针对我国学者主张脑死亡立法的主要论据,提出并分析了脑死亡问题的三个主要问题:(1)脑死亡是真正的死亡吗?(2)能否以经济效率的考量来定义生死?(3)器官移植和脑死亡到底有什么关系?还提出,脑死亡立法需要在我国现实的医疗卫生条件下进行讨论。 相似文献
222.
Gaillard V Barrouillet P Jarrold C Camos V 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2011,110(3):469-479
Several models assume that working memory development depends on age-related increases in efficiency and speed of processing. However, age-related increases in the efficiency of the mechanisms that counteract forgetting and restore memory traces may also be important. This hypothesis was tested in three experiments by manipulating both the processing duration within a working memory task and the time available to restore memory traces. Third- and sixth-grade children performed a complex span task in which they maintained series of letters while adding numbers to series of digits. When we equated processing and restoration times between ages, the developmental difference in working memory span was reduced but remained significant. However, this residual difference was eliminated when the time available to reactivate memory traces was tailored to the processing speed of each age group. This indicates that children employ active mechanisms for maintenance and restoration of memory traces that develop with age. 相似文献
223.
回顾性分析SLIPA^TM通气道(喉罩)临床应用的优缺点及相关并发症。330例ASA1-3级各类全麻手术患者,快速诱导麻醉后插入SLIPA^TM通气道。记录脉搏血氧饱和度,呼气末二氧化碳分压,插入的难易程度及是否有反流、误吸和术后咽痛等相关并发症。结果为96%的患者容易插入SLIPA^TM通气道,10.9%的患者发生了一定程度胃胀气,血液痕迹残留SLIPA^TM通气道表面发生率为11.5%,术后咽痛的发生率为3.3%,无一例发生术后肺内感染。SLIPA^TM通气道操作方便,通气可靠,围术期并发症少,在临床的应用安全有效。 相似文献
224.
Julien Chardonnens Julien Favre Florian Cuendet Gérald Gremion Kamiar Aminian 《Human movement science》2013,32(4):741-752
Take-off, the most important phase in ski jumping, has been primarily studied in terms of spatio-temporal parameters; little is known about its motor control aspects. This study aims to assess the inter-segment coordination of the shank-thigh and thigh-sacrum pairs using the continuous relative phase (CRP). In total 87 jumps were recorded from 33 athletes with an inertial sensor-based system. The CRP curves indicated that the thighs lead the shanks during the first part of take-off extension and that the shanks rotated faster at the take-off extension end. The thighs and sacrum first rotated synchronously, with the sacrum then taking lead, with finally the thighs rotating faster. Five characteristic features were extracted from the CRP and their relationship with jump length was tested. Three features of the shank-thigh pair and one of the thigh-sacrum pair reported a significant association with jump length. It was observed that athletes who achieved longer jumps had their thighs leading their shanks during a longer time, with these athletes also having a more symmetric movement between thighs and sacrum. This study shows that inter-segment coordination during the take-off extension is related to performance and further studies are necessary to contrast its importance with other ski jumping aspects. 相似文献
225.
Undergraduate university students (N = 103) participated in a study of the relationship between test anxiety and the variables of trait anxiety, self-esteem, locus of control, mental ability, and gender. Results indicated bivariate associations between total test anxiety and the other measures except for mental ability. Further analyses revealed independent relationships between the “worry” component of test anxiety and the variables of trait anxiety, intemality, chance, and mental ability. We also found independent associations between the “emotionality” aspect of test anxiety and the measures of trait anxiety and chance. 相似文献
226.
Grilo CM 《Behaviour research and therapy》2004,42(1):57-65
This study examined the diagnostic efficiency of the DSM-IV criteria for obsessive compulsive personality disorder (OCPD) in patients with binge eating disorder (BED). Two hundred and eleven consecutive adult patients with axis I diagnoses of BED were reliably assessed with semi-structured diagnostic interviews. Conditional probabilities-sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive power (PPP), and negative predictive power (NPP)-were calculated for each of the eight criteria for OCPD, using the 'best-estimate' OCPD diagnosis as the standard. The diagnostic efficiencies of the OCPD criteria were variable, with three criteria failing to have predictive value (PPP<0.50). The best inclusion criterion (highest PPP) was 'Perfectionism,' which was also the overall most predictive criterion. The findings suggest ordering of the DSM-IV criteria for OCPD based on performance and call into question the utility of some criteria. 相似文献
227.
Stephan C. Meylan;Thomas L. Griffiths; 《Cognitive Science》2024,48(7):e13478
How do cognitive pressures shape the lexicons of natural languages? Here, we reframe George Kingsley Zipf's proposed “law of abbreviation” within a more general framework that relates it to cognitive pressures that affect speakers and listeners. In this new framework, speakers' drive to reduce effort (Zipf's proposal) is counteracted by the need for low-frequency words to have word forms that are sufficiently distinctive to allow for accurate recognition by listeners. To support this framework, we replicate and extend recent work using the prevalence of subword phonemic sequences (phonotactic probability) to measure speakers' production effort in place of Zipf's measure of length. Across languages and corpora, phonotactic probability is more strongly correlated with word frequency than word length. We also show this measure of ease of speech production (phonotactic probability) is strongly correlated with a measure of perceptual difficulty that indexes the degree of competition from alternative interpretations in word recognition. This is consistent with the claim that there must be trade-offs between these two factors, and is inconsistent with a recent proposal that phonotactic probability facilitates both perception and production. To our knowledge, this is the first work to offer an explanation why long, phonotactically improbable word forms remain in the lexicons of natural languages. 相似文献
228.
Paul Bertin;Octavia Ionescu;Ricky Green;Koen Abts;Julius Rogenhofer;Sylvain Delouvée;Vincent Yzerbyt;Olivier Klein; 《European journal of social psychology》2024,54(6):1393-1415
Conspiracy beliefs entail a scapegoating function by attributing the consequences of crises, such as economic downturns, to the secret action of outgroups. While conspiracy beliefs have been described as reactions to economic threats, we argue that this factor alone is not sufficient. Rather, perceiving one's ingroup as unfairly deprived compared to other groups (i.e., group relative deprivation) might be key to explaining the situation in terms of secret, intentional wrongdoings. Furthermore, individuals high in national narcissism (i.e., a perceived lack of recognition of the ingroup's greatness), may be especially sensitive to this dynamic. Three pilot studies (N = 1237) attested the robustness of the link between group relative deprivation and conspiracy beliefs. Then, Study 1 (N = 812) revealed that the effect of group relative deprivation on conspiracy beliefs was moderated by national narcissism. In Study 2 (N = 728), we found effects of induced national narcissism and group relative deprivation on conspiracy beliefs in a fictitious setting. Study 3 (N = 846) replicated the moderation of national narcissism on the link between group relative deprivation and conspiracy beliefs at the cross-sectional level. Overall, these studies provide evidence that conspiracy beliefs in reaction to group relative deprivation are especially likely among collective narcissists. We discuss the scapegoating function of conspiracy beliefs during crises. 相似文献
229.
Moral disengagement is an important aggressive and moral cognition. The mechanisms of changes in moral disengagement remain unclear, especially at the within-person level. We attempted to clarify this by exploring the serial effects of personal relative deprivation and hostility on civic moral disengagement. We conducted a three-wave longitudinal survey with 1058 undergraduates (63.61% women; mean age = 20.97). The results of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model showed that personal relative deprivation at Wave 1 and hostility at Wave 2 formed a serial effect on the within-person changes in civic moral disengagement at Wave 3, and the longitudinal indirect effect test showed that the within-person dynamics in hostility at Wave 2 acted as a mediator. The results of multiple group analysis across genders further showed that the longitudinal indirect role of hostility at Wave 2 was only observed for men, but not for women, which indicates the moderating effect of gender. These findings facilitate an understanding of the mechanisms of aggressive cognitions at the within-person level and offer implications for the prevention and intervention of aggression from the perspective of moral cognition. 相似文献
230.
Joshua Jessel Rachel Metras Gregory P. Hanley Catherine Jessel Einar T. Ingvarsson 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2020,53(1):25-43
We conducted this study to determine if the efficiency of the functional analysis could be improved without detrimental effects on control. In Experiment 1, we reanalyzed functional analyses conducted for the problem behavior of 18 children. We analyzed rates of problem behavior during the first 5 min and first 3 min of the original 10-min sessions and evaluated if changes in the level of control over problem behavior by the programmed contingency were evident from the analyses of shorter session duration. In Experiment 2, we conducted 8 consecutive functional analyses with 3-min sessions to further evaluate the utility of brief session durations. We found that control over problem behavior was demonstrated when conducting functional analyses with sessions as brief as 3 min. 相似文献