全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7289篇 |
免费 | 470篇 |
国内免费 | 56篇 |
专业分类
7815篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 62篇 |
2023年 | 82篇 |
2022年 | 24篇 |
2021年 | 187篇 |
2020年 | 286篇 |
2019年 | 727篇 |
2018年 | 571篇 |
2017年 | 709篇 |
2016年 | 658篇 |
2015年 | 544篇 |
2014年 | 484篇 |
2013年 | 696篇 |
2012年 | 352篇 |
2011年 | 298篇 |
2010年 | 232篇 |
2009年 | 235篇 |
2008年 | 184篇 |
2007年 | 206篇 |
2006年 | 182篇 |
2005年 | 163篇 |
2004年 | 141篇 |
2003年 | 149篇 |
2002年 | 121篇 |
2001年 | 116篇 |
2000年 | 89篇 |
1999年 | 51篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 15篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 8篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有7815条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
Previous studies have noted that writing processes are impaired by task-irrelevant background sound. However, what makes sound distracting to writing processes has remained unaddressed. The experiment reported here investigated whether the semanticity of irrelevant speech contributes to disruption of writing processes beyond the acoustic properties of the sound. The participants wrote stories against a background of normal speech, spectrally-rotated speech (i.e., a meaningless sound with marked acoustic resemblance to speech) or silence. Normal speech impaired quantitative (e.g., number of characters produced) and qualitative/semantic (e.g., uncorrected typing errors, proposition generation) aspects of the written material, in comparison with the other two sound conditions, and it increased the duration of pauses between words. No difference was found between the silent and the rotated-speech condition. These results suggest that writing is susceptible to disruption from the semanticity of speech but not especially susceptible to disruption from the acoustic properties of speech. 相似文献
22.
《Counselling psychology quarterly》2012,25(3):263-275
Aim: This study explores the author's experience of coding a single session of her therapeutic practice with a client for her therapeutic intentions. Method and analysis: A session of counselling was audio-recorded, transcribed and coded for intentions using the Therapist Intentions List [Hill, C.E., & O’Grady, K.E. (1985). List of therapist intentions illustrated in a case study and with therapists of varying theoretical orientations. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32, 3–22]. Following this, the trainee's journal entry focusing on the training exercise was analysed using tools from the grounded theory approach [Corbin, J.M., & Strauss, A.L. (2008). Basics of qualitative research. Techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory. London: Sage]. Findings: A core category of “Reflective learning: a process of ‘self-supervision’” was generated with four subcategories: (1) “experience of audio-recording a client session”; (2) “experience of transcribing a client session”; (3) “experience of coding for intentions”; and (4) “further reflections beyond the training exercise.” Discussion: The findings are consistent with literature suggesting that engaging in exercises promoting reflection on practice is experienced as beneficial in training. Furthermore, they are broadly consistent with the suggestion that transcribing and coding a session of counselling and reflecting on one's therapeutic intentions in a paper can be a “profound training experience” [Hill, C.E., Stahl, J., & Roffman, M. (2007). Training novice psychotherapists: Helping skills and beyond. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 44, 364–370, p. 368]. 相似文献
23.
Two studies using novel extensions of the conditioned head-turning method examined contributions of rhythmic and distributional properties of syllable strings to 8-month-old infants' speech segmentation. The two techniques introduced exploit fundamental, but complementary, properties of representational units. The first involved assessment of discriminative response maintenance when simple training stimuli were embedded in more complex speech contexts; the second involved measurement of infants' latencies in detecting extraneous signals superimposed on speech stimuli. A complex pattern of results is predicted if infants succeed in grouping syllables into higher-order units. Across the two studies, the predicted pattern of results emerged, indicating that rhythmic properties of speech play an important role in guiding infants toward potential linguistically relevant units and simultaneously demonstrating that the techniques proposed here provide valid, converging measures of infants' auditory representational units. 相似文献
24.
Martin Schoemann Michael Schulte‐Mecklenbeck Frank Renkewitz Stefan Scherbaum 《决策行为杂志》2019,32(5):521-535
The study of cognitive processes is built on a close mapping between three components: overt gaze behavior, overt choice, and covert processes. To validate this overt–covert mapping in the domain of decision‐making, we collected eye‐movement data during decisions between risky gamble problems. Applying a forward inference paradigm, participants were instructed to use specific decision strategies to solve those gamble problems (maximizing expected values or applying different choice heuristics) during which gaze behavior was recorded. We revealed differences between overt behavior, as indicated by eye movements, and covert decision processes, instructed by the experimenter. However, our results show that the overt–covert mapping is for some eye‐movement measures not as close as expected by current decision theory, and hence question reverse inference as being prone to fallacies due to a violation of its prerequisite, that is, a close overt–covert mapping. We propose a framework to rehabilitate reverse inference. 相似文献
25.
26.
5──8岁儿童对几种偏正复句的理解 总被引:3,自引:2,他引:3
本研究用三种方式测试80名5—8岁儿童对因果、条件和让步三种复句的理解。结果表明,他们对这三种复句的理解逐年提高,6岁和7岁之间的变化尤为明显。6岁儿童基本能理解因果复句和条件复句,7岁儿童基本能理解让步复句。他们在三种作业中表现出不同的理解水平,判断句子正误的成绩差于完成句子和指认代词所指的成绩。 相似文献
27.
Identification With All Humanity (IWAH) relates to higher levels of concern and supportive behavior toward the disadvantaged, stronger endorsement of human rights, and stronger responses in favor of global harmony. So far, IWAH has been conceptualized as a one‐dimensional construct describing the degree with which one identifies with all humans as a superordinate ingroup. However, recent group identification models suggest a multi‐dimensional model to provide a more differentiated approach toward the understanding of the highest level of social identification. Using principal axis (Study 1) and confirmatory (Study 2) factor analyses, we suggest that IWAH sub‐divides into two dimensions—global self‐definition and global self‐investment. Study 2 revealed that global self‐investment was a stronger predictor for both convergent measures (e.g., social dominance orientation and authoritarianism) and behavioral intentions than global self‐definition. Finally, in Study 3, we manipulated IWAH to test its causal effect on donation behavior. Participants in the experimental condition, compared with the control condition, showed higher global self‐investment, which in turn predicted greater giving to global charity. These findings suggest that two dimensions with different behavioral outcomes underlie IWAH. 相似文献
28.
Test anxiety (TA) is a prevalent issue among students that can result in deleterious consequences, such as underachievement. However, a contemporary measure that has been validated for use with Australian students seems to be lacking. This study, therefore, investigated the suitability of the German Test Anxiety Inventory (TAI‐G) for use with Australian university students. While the original TAI‐G contains 30 items and was designed to measure four factors (worry, emotionality, interference, and lack of confidence), differing factorial models have been supported in the literature using either the original or a shortened 17‐item version of the measure. These differing TAI‐G models were tested and compared in the current study via confirmatory factor analysis using 224 Australian university students. As expected, results supported the superior fit of the 17‐item four‐factor model. Additionally, the convergent validity of the measure was supported since measures of self‐esteem, self‐efficacy, and general anxiety were all found to correlate significantly with the TAI‐G in the hypothesised directions. Finally, the finding that all of the TAI‐G subscales had acceptably high reliabilities led to the conclusion that the 17‐item TAI‐G is a valid and reliable measure of TA in an Australian university population. 相似文献
29.
30.
The Self‐Care of Psychologists and Mental Health Professionals: A Review and Practitioner Guide
下载免费PDF全文

Frank M Dattilio 《Australian psychologist》2015,50(6):393-399
It has been documented in the professional literature that psychologists and mental health professionals in general tend to neglect their own mental health, despite serving in a field that promotes the health and well‐being of others. Numerous empirical studies support the need for psychologists and mental health professionals to be more conscientious of the effect that stress and the nature of their work has on them. This article reviews some of the recent literature and discusses the pertinent issues at hand. A number of interventions are suggested along with tips for professionals to consider in order to function in a more productive and ethical fashion. 相似文献