首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   204篇
  免费   22篇
  国内免费   8篇
  2024年   1篇
  2022年   1篇
  2021年   5篇
  2020年   4篇
  2019年   6篇
  2018年   9篇
  2017年   10篇
  2016年   5篇
  2015年   1篇
  2014年   3篇
  2013年   20篇
  2012年   5篇
  2011年   5篇
  2010年   7篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   7篇
  2007年   14篇
  2006年   11篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   10篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   7篇
  2000年   2篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   3篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   6篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   6篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   4篇
  1985年   4篇
  1984年   5篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   5篇
  1981年   4篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   4篇
  1977年   3篇
  1976年   1篇
排序方式: 共有234条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
101.
The internal processes involved in synchronizing our movements with environmental stimuli have traditionally been addressed using regular metronomic sequences. Regarding real-life environments, however, biological rhythms are known to have intrinsic variability, ubiquitously characterized as fractal long-range correlations. In our research we thus investigate to what extent the synchronization processes drawn from regular metronome paradigms can be generalized to other (biologically) variable rhythms. Participants performed synchronized finger tapping under five conditions of long-range and/or short-range correlated, randomly variable, and regular auditory sequences. Combining experimental data analysis and numerical simulation, we found that synchronizing with biologically variable rhythms involves the same internal processes as with other variable rhythms (whether totally random or comprising lawful regularities), but different from those involved with a regular metronome. This challenges both the generalizability of conclusions drawn from regular-metronome paradigms, and recent research assuming that biologically variable rhythms may trigger specific strong anticipatory processes to achieve synchronization.  相似文献   
102.
A 2 × 2 chi-square can be computed from a phi coefficient, which is the Pearson correlation between two binomial variables. Similarly, chi-square for larger contingency tables can be computed from canonical correlation coefficients. The authors address the following series of issues involving this relationship: (a) how to represent a contingency table in terms of a correlation matrix involving r - 1 row and c - 1 column dummy predictors; (b) how to compute chi-square from canonical correlations solved from this matrix; (c) how to compute loadings for the omitted row and column variables; and (d) the possible interpretive advantage of describing canonical relationships that comprise chi-square, together with some examples. The proposed procedures integrate chi-square analysis of contingency tables with general correlational theory and serve as an introduction to some recent methods of analysis more widely known by sociologists.  相似文献   
103.
The association structure between manifest variables arising from the single-factor model is investigated using partial correlations. The additional insights to the practitioner provided by partial correlations for detecting a single-factor model are discussed. The parameter space for the partial correlations is presented, as are the patterns of signs in a matrix containing the partial correlations that are not compatible with a single-factor model.  相似文献   
104.
The paper argues that mental causation can be explained from the sufficiency of counterfactual dependence for causation together with relatively weak assumptions about the metaphysics of mind. If a physical event counterfactually depends on an earlier physical event, it also counterfactually depends on, and hence is caused by, a mental event that correlates with (or supervenes on) this earlier physical event, provided that this correlation (or supervenience) is sufficiently modally robust. This account of mental causation is consistent with the overdetermination of physical events by mental events and other physical events, but does not entail it.
Thomas KroedelEmail:
  相似文献   
105.
双生子及子代研究(TOSS)收集了909对双生子及他(她)们的配偶和他(她)们的青少年子女的数据。设计该项目是为研究以下三个问题:1)遗传与环境是如何影响双生子父母的养育方式的?2)遗传与环境是如何影响成年人的家庭关系的?3)家庭关系是如何影响成年人的心理调适的?由于此研究包括了双生子及其子女,该设计可以与其他研究相比较来具体地分析遗传与环境的关联。此研究的结果有助于厘清遗传与环境在养育方式和家庭关系中的型态,同时也澄清了父母亲个人特征是如何影响他(她)们的养育方式,以及父母的个体特征是如何帮助解释了遗传对家庭关系的影响。此研究今后将进一步分析遗传与环境的关联与互动,以及候选基因与环境的关联与互动  相似文献   
106.
探讨了当相关系数对变量之间相关程度不易进行度量或者无法度量的情况下,是否可以采用关联规则方法来考察变量之间的相互关系。从理论上分析了关联规则方法在心理测量中应用的可能性,并运用Apriori算法挖掘频繁项集,对来华留学生跨文化适应性数据进行了分析,将关联规则方法分析结果与Pearson相关、Spearman相关结果进行比较,发现当使用关联规则方法进行相互关系分析时,能够更加简易、有效地获得变量间的相关规则。研究结果表明关联规则方法可以获得变量各个水平具体的支持度和置信度,推导出多变量之间的相关规则,并且对于处理非线性的多变量以及多分类变量的相关是非常有用的方法。  相似文献   
107.
运用联想测试方法探索高二学生学习新陈代谢一章后的认知结构。运用相关系数值绘制出了学生的认知结构图。结果显示,运用概念图策略进行教学的实验班学生更容易形成认知结构。实验班学生所形成的认知结构更良好、更合理。  相似文献   
108.
石文典  肖余春 《心理科学》2004,27(4):986-989
在间隔两个月重复测试我国城市居民消费心理预期水准的基础上,采用交叉滞后相关法(CLPC)研究了消费心理预期水准与股票涨跌幅之间的关系.发现:消费心理预期是导致股票涨跌的主要因素.从而证明消费心理预期是影响消费需求的重要因素。  相似文献   
109.
概化理论研究及应用前景   总被引:9,自引:0,他引:9  
刘桔 《心理科学》2003,26(3):433-437
1972年,Cronbach和他的同事们提出概化理论之后,概化理论在行为与心理测量领域得到了广泛的应用,较之经典测量理论,它的优势逐渐地显露:(1)测量的多种误差来源可以在同一个分析中分别估计;(2)可以指导决策者选择最优测量方案;(3)提供可靠性系数:概化系数(G系数)和依存性指标(φ系数)用于不同的决策任务;(4)排除了严格平行测验的假设。概化理论以它的精确性和可藏性受到了信度测量领域研究者们的青睐,本文旨在对概化理论的基本框架、产生、发展及应用前景进行详细论述。  相似文献   
110.
Studies find a strong positive relationship between the affective components of anxiety and depression. However, most research thus far has examined the between-person correlations among these constructs, while ignoring how changes in these two types of affect covary over time within a person. Within-person correlations could differ meaningfully from how anxiety- and depression-related affect relate across individuals. Further, individuals may differ in terms of how highly these constructs covary over time. The current study aimed to (1) compare between- and within-person correlations between anxious and depressive affect, (2) examine lagged effects between anxious and depressive affect over time, (3) test whether individuals differ in their within-person correlations between these two types of affect, and (4) examine whether the mean level of affective intensity moderated these individual differences. These questions were explored using college undergraduates (N?=?50) who rated their depression- and anxiety- related affect six times a day for two weeks. A higher average correlation was observed between anxious and depressive affect in between-person compared to within-person analyses. Significant bidirectional lagged effects were observed between these constructs. Individuals with higher average levels of anxious affect experienced stronger within-person correlations between anxious and depressive affect.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号