首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   183篇
  免费   25篇
  国内免费   25篇
  2023年   3篇
  2022年   2篇
  2021年   7篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   10篇
  2017年   7篇
  2016年   9篇
  2015年   10篇
  2014年   3篇
  2013年   30篇
  2012年   11篇
  2011年   5篇
  2010年   11篇
  2009年   7篇
  2008年   8篇
  2007年   8篇
  2006年   14篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   12篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   5篇
  2001年   5篇
  2000年   5篇
  1999年   4篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   3篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   1篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   2篇
  1985年   2篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
排序方式: 共有233条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
The static dielectric constants of binary mixtures of N-methylformamide (NMF), used as a solute with the solvents water (W), ethyl alcohol (EA), ethylene glycol (EG), dimethyl sulphoxide (DMSO), acetone (Ac) and 1,4-dioxane (Dx), have been measured over the entire concentration range at 30°C. The negative values of excess dielectric constant and less-than-unity values of the corrective Kirkwood correlation factor of these binary mixtures are used to analyse the formation of H-bond complexes and molar ratio of the stable adduct. In these systems, the unassociated mixture constituents, DMSO, Ac and Dx, act as structure-breakers for self-associated linear structures of NMF, whereas, both the self-associated mixture constituents (W, EA and EG mixtures with NMF) act as structure-breakers to each other during the complexations. It is found that the large variations in the strength of H-bond complexation and the effective number of parallel aligned dipoles in the mixtures are governed by the dielectric constant of the solvents.  相似文献   
152.
In this study, the relationship between a self-report measure of assertive behavior in children, the Children's Assertiveness Inventory, and a role-play measure of assertive behavior, the Revised Behavioral Assertiveness Test for Children, was examined in 69 elementary school children. Measures of assertive responding to positive (initiating interactions/giving and receiving compliments) and negative (standing up for one's own rights/refusing unreasonable requests) situations were obtained for both self-report and role-play measures. Results suggest that self-report of positive assertion is more closely related to role-play measures and expert ratings of assertiveness than is self-report of negative assertion. Results are discussed in terms of relationship between assertive behavior and aggressive behavior in children and the need to "unbind" these two notions in future studies. Developmental issues which limit the finings are also addressed.  相似文献   
153.
The effect of a fixed-time (FT) schedule involving the delivery of preferred stimuli prior to the issuance of a low-probability instruction was evaluated with 2 young children with autism. The FT schedule was introduced according to a reversal design with 3 target instructions, 1 for the first child and 2 for the second child. Compliance increased for 2 of the 3 cases. A high-probability instruction sequence and guided compliance were implemented for the second instruction targeted for 1 child, with compliance increasing with guided compliance.  相似文献   
154.
"先天八卦"中具有数理内涵,同样,"后天八卦"中亦具有数理内涵。前者呈二进制形态,后者则为三进制形态。《系辞》曰:"太极生两仪,两仪生四象,四象生八卦。"其实,四象生八卦有两种不同的逻辑法则,这两种不同的逻辑法则导致二进制八卦与三进制八卦。而且"后天八卦"与五行具有内在的关联,因此,"后天八卦"中的数理内涵比"先天八卦"中的数理内涵更显丰富。  相似文献   
155.
采用序列学习研究范式,考察小三到大三学生内隐序列学习的发展规律及制约因素。结果表明:(1)各年龄组被试均能在序列学习中获得序列规则;(2)不同年龄组被试序列反应时存在显著差异,说明内隐序列学习从小三到大三之间一直存在发展变化;(3)被试在高统计结构下的反应时要低于低统计结构下的,且这种差异不存在发展变化,说明高统计结构下的内隐学习成绩要好于低统计结构下的;(4)外显被试的量随年龄增长而增加,说明对序列学习的有意识加工能力随年龄增长而提高。  相似文献   
156.
The present study aimed to investigate whether an interference task might impact the sleep-dependent consolidation process of a mentally learned sequence of movements. Thirty-two participants were subjected to a first training session through motor imagery (MI) or physical practice (PP) of a finger sequence learning task. After 2 h, half of the participants were requested to perform a second interfering PP task (reversed finger sequence). All participants were finally re-tested following a night of sleep on the first finger sequence. The main findings revealed delayed performance gains following a night of sleep in the MI group, i.e. the interfering task did not alter the consolidation process, by contrast to the PP group. These results confirm that MI practice might result in less retroactive interference than PP, and further highlight the relevance of the first night of sleep for the consolidation process following MI practice. These data might thus contribute to determine in greater details the practical implications of mental training in motor learning and rehabilitation.  相似文献   
157.
中小学生内隐序列学习的机制   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
内隐序列学习是内隐学习的一种重要形式。该文探讨了中小学生内隐序列学习的心理机制及次级任务影响内隐序列学习的实质。结果表明,中小学生对不同结构序列的内隐学习是由一个单一的学习机制完成的,学习的过程即是形成序列内相邻元素间联合的过程;另外,注意能量并不是内隐序列学习的重要影响因素,次级任务影响内隐序列学习并不是因为其分散了注意能量,而是破坏了对序列元素的组织。  相似文献   
158.
知识类别和特点对内隐序列学习的影响   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
用序列学习中的反应时和错误个数间接测量被试的内隐知识,用再认成绩和预测成绩直接测量被试的外显知识,通过倒转不同的规则探讨了知识类别和特点对内隐序列学习的影响。结果表明:(1)内隐学习和外显学习可能分别依赖于两个独立的学习系统,且互不干扰;(2)当内隐序列学习既包含运动知识又包含概念知识时,知识类别和特点影响被试对序列知识的获得,被试较易获得运动知识;(3)在内隐序列学习中练习的数量也影响被试对序列知识的获得,序列学习是一个动态的过程。  相似文献   
159.
160.
We report three studies demonstrating the ‘lure of choice’ people prefer options that allow them to take further choices over those that do not, even when the extra choices cannot improve the ultimate outcome. In Studies 1 and 2, participants chose between two options: one solitary item, and a pair of items between which they would then make a further choice. Consistent with the lure of choice, a given item was more likely to be the ultimate choice when it was initially part of a choice pair than when it was offered on its own. We also demonstrate the lure of choice in a four‐door version of the Monty Hall problem, in which participants could either stick with their original choice or switch to one of two unopened doors. Participants were more likely to switch if they could first ‘choose to choose’ between the two unopened doors (without immediately specifying which) than if they had to choose one door straightaway. We conclude by suggesting that the lure of choice is due to a choice heuristic that is very reliable in the natural world, but much less so in a world created by marketers. Copyright © 2003 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号