全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3129篇 |
免费 | 463篇 |
国内免费 | 149篇 |
专业分类
3741篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 7篇 |
2024年 | 47篇 |
2023年 | 56篇 |
2022年 | 80篇 |
2021年 | 115篇 |
2020年 | 149篇 |
2019年 | 211篇 |
2018年 | 160篇 |
2017年 | 197篇 |
2016年 | 191篇 |
2015年 | 105篇 |
2014年 | 168篇 |
2013年 | 601篇 |
2012年 | 143篇 |
2011年 | 194篇 |
2010年 | 101篇 |
2009年 | 157篇 |
2008年 | 165篇 |
2007年 | 137篇 |
2006年 | 98篇 |
2005年 | 109篇 |
2004年 | 109篇 |
2003年 | 93篇 |
2002年 | 89篇 |
2001年 | 53篇 |
2000年 | 38篇 |
1999年 | 21篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 28篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有3741条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
Jutta Schickore 《Journal for General Philosophy of Science》1999,30(2):273-287
Vision, Visibility, and Empirical Research. In general, natural scientists use the concept of observation in a liberal way:
they talk of observing electrons, DNA, or distant quasars. Several philosophers of science have recently argued for a similar
use of the concept of observation: they have claimed that the important aspects of scientific research can only be properly
reconstructed in accordance with how this term is actually used in science. With reference to an example from astronomy, I
point out that the proposed generalisation of the concept of observation leads to undesirable consequences. I argue that a
differentiated conceptual framework is required in order to give an adequate account of the varieties of scientific experience.
Thus, the appropriate starting point for distinguishing these various scientific research practices should not be the generalised
scientific conception of observation, but instead distinctly different uses of the term observation drawn from ordinary language.
This revised version was published online in August 2006 with corrections to the Cover Date. 相似文献
52.
P. van Donkelaar G. M. Gauthier J.-L. Vercher J. Blouin 《Journal of motor behavior》2013,45(4):315-323
The gain of ocular smooth pursuit responses can be adaptively modified under certain circumstances. Evidence that these modifications are caused, at least partly, by changes at a sensory level comes from the fact that subsequent manual tracking movements generated during visual fixation are also modified in a similar manner. The question addressed in the present experiment was whether the modifications are constrained specifically to responses such as these, which are driven by retinal image motion, or whether they can also influence movements that make use of retinal image position. This was accomplished by comparing manual and oculomotor responses to step changes in target position before and after smooth pursuit adaptation. The results showed that, as with manual tracking movements, these responses were also modified by the adaptive procedure, although to a lesser extent. In particular, following a 20-min period in which subjects (N = 4) were submitted to a procedure designed to increase the gain of the smooth pursuit system, manual step tracking movements and ocular saccades displayed larger amplitudes than those shown prior to the adaptation. In addition, these gain changes were accompanied by appropriate alterations in response kinematics. Taken together, these results suggest that the mechanisms responsible for adaptive modifications in the smooth pursuit system are also able to more generally influence the processing of visuospatial information. The possible neurophysiological substrate underlying these mechanisms is discussed. 相似文献
53.
This study tests whether the face-processing system of humans and a nonhuman primate species share characteristics that would allow for early and quick processing of socially salient stimuli: a sensitivity toward conspecific faces, a sensitivity toward highly practiced face stimuli, and an ability to generalize changes in the face that do not suggest a new identity, such as a face differently oriented. The look rates by adult tamarins and humans toward conspecific and other primate faces were examined to determine if these characteristics are shared. A visual paired comparison (VPC) task presented subjects with either a human face, chimpanzee face, tamarin face, or an object as a sample, and then a pair containing the previous stimulus and a novel stimulus was presented. The stimuli were either presented all in an upright orientation, or all in an inverted orientation. The novel stimulus in the pair was either an orientation change of the same face/object or a new example of the same type of face/object, and the stimuli were shown either in an upright orientation or in an inverted orientation. Preference to novelty scores revealed that humans attended most to novel individual human faces, and this effect decreased significantly if the stimuli were inverted. Tamarins showed preferential looking toward novel orientations of previously seen tamarin faces in the upright orientation, but not in an inverted orientation. Similarly, their preference to look longer at novel tamarin and human faces within the pair was reduced significantly with inverted stimuli. The results confirmed prior findings in humans that novel human faces generate more attention in the upright than in the inverted orientation. The monkeys also attended more to faces of conspecifics, but showed an inversion effect to orientation change in tamarin faces and to identity changes in tamarin and human faces. The results indicate configural processing restricted to particular kinds of primate faces by a New World monkey species, with configural processing influenced by life experience (human faces and tamarin faces) and specialized to process orientation changes specific to conspecific faces. 相似文献
54.
古琴打谱是一项专业性很强又费时费力的工作,如何利用先进的人工智能技术来对古琴谱中的谱字进行自动识别解读,哪怕是辅助性的,对于古琴打谱事业的发展无疑会起到重要的促进作用,从可以间接地为保护与弘扬古琴文化作贡献。本文通过研究古琴减字谱这种特殊文本的特点,提出了不同于普通OCR光学识别软件的文本切分方法,内容涉及纸质古琴谱原始扫描图的图像预处理,古琴混合谱中简字谱的行切分与提取,单行简字谱中的谱字切分与提取等算法及其实现。这样就为实现古琴谱进一步的释读提供一种前期处理方法。由于古琴谱字的特殊性,以及所提出方法的通用性,该方法对于丰富汉字文本的切分方法也有着一定的学术意义。 相似文献
55.
The basal ganglia and cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical connections are known to play a critical role in sequence skill learning and increasing automaticity over practice. The current paper reviews four studies comparing the sequence skill learning and the transition to automaticity of persons who stutter (PWS) and fluent speakers (PNS) over practice. Studies One and Two found PWS to have poor finger tap sequencing skill and nonsense syllable sequencing skill after practice, and on retention and transfer tests relative to PNS. Studies Three and Four found PWS to be significantly less accurate and/or significantly slower after practice on dual tasks requiring concurrent sequencing and colour recognition over practice relative to PNS. Evidence of PWS’ deficits in sequence skill learning and automaticity development support the hypothesis that dysfunction in cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical connections may be one etiological component in the development and maintenance of stuttering.
Educational objectives: As a result of this activity, the reader will: (1) be able to articulate the research regarding the basal ganglia system relating to sequence skill learning; (2) be able to summarize the research on stuttering with indications of sequence skill learning deficits; and (3) be able to discuss basal ganglia mechanisms with relevance for theory of stuttering. 相似文献
56.
选取条件概率(P(Q|P))由低到高的四个命题作为四卡问题中的检验规则,探讨了大学生被试对四张卡片的逻辑证明作用的推断能力及其对解决四卡问题的影响。结果发现:(1)不同条件概率的命题之间正确选择P-Q的人数百分比不存在显著差异,命题的条件概率因素对四卡问题的正确解决没有影响。(2)逻辑分析过程对四卡问题的正确解决产生了一定的抑制作用,这可能是因为被试不能从整体上思考四张卡片在命题检验中的逻辑作用的缘故。(3)一些被试即使在逻辑分析过程中表现出知道-Q卡片的证伪作用,仍然倾向于选择卡片Q而非-Q,这一现象再次证实了人类思维的非形式逻辑的一面。 相似文献
57.
本研究借鉴Mitchell等人的图词归源和早晚归源任务,引发启发式和系统式加工,并引入知觉流畅性变量,试图在知觉层面上分离两种加工的底层工作模式。结果发现:(1)源检测具有相对脆弱性,成绩显著差于再认。(2)启发式加工和系统式加工在加工速度和底层工作模式上分离,启发式加工速度快,其工作模式似乎是对刺激的知觉流畅性进行阈下水平的解释与归因,而系统式加工速度慢,其工作模式似乎是对意义的分析与推理。 相似文献
58.
选取轻度弱智、一般智力以及智力优秀三组共288名小学生作为被试,每个被试接受三种测验,即工作记忆测验、加工速度测验、韦氏儿童智力测验中国修订版,以考察轻度弱智儿童群体在与工作记忆和加工速度相关的认知任务上的反应特征,以及轻度弱智儿童与一般儿童在这些任务上的差异。通过方差分析、聚类分析等统计方法得到以下结果:1.轻度弱智儿童和智力一般儿童在工作记忆和加工速度上有显著差异。2.轻度弱智儿童群体在各基本认知能力中可能存在的缺陷并不平衡,工作记忆较稳定地反映了该群体在智力上的缺陷,但存在一部分在加工速度上体现为正常水平的亚类群体。 相似文献
59.
本研究通过三个实验对Lillard的Moe假装任务进行了重复与改进,旨在探讨儿童对假装行为中知识状态作用的理解,进而讨论儿童对假装的心理本质的理解。实验一表明,“是否”问题使得5岁儿童在该任务上表现出肯定回答倾向;实验二表明.当改变提问方式,在问题中提供选择项目后,5岁儿童在该任务上的通过率有显著提高.肯定回答倾向明显减弱;实验三表明,当减少指导语中相互矛盾的信息、明确指出主人公知道某物而不知道另一物以及减弱对未知状态和知道状态的强调的情况下.5岁儿童能根据主人公所知道的事物来推断出其行为是在假装成该事物,能理解到知识状态在假装行为中的作用。这一结果表明5岁儿童能够理解到假装中所隐含的心理表征,Lillard所提出的Moe任务范式低估了儿童对假装的心理本质的理解能力。 相似文献
60.
Alerting enhances attentional bias for salient stimuli: Evidence from a global/local processing task
The present study examined the role of alerting in modulating attentional bias to salient events. In a global/local processing task, participants were presented with a large arrow (global level) comprised of smaller arrows (local level) pointing in the same or opposite directions and had to indicate the direction of the large or small arrows in different blocks. Saliency of the global and local levels was manipulated, creating global-salient and local-salient conditions. Alerting signals were presented in half of the trials prior to the target. Results revealed a double dissociation in the effects of alerting on global/local interference effects. In a global salient condition, alerting increased global interference and decreased local interference. In a local salient condition, alerting reduced global interference and increased local interference. We demonstrate that within a single task, alerting can increase and reduce conflict based on perceptual saliency. These findings help to better understand disorders like hemispatial neglect in which both arousal and attention to salient events are impaired. These results also challenge previous theories suggesting that alerting acts to increase conflict interference. We argue that alerting is an adaptive mechanism that diverts attention to salient events, but comes at a cost when selective attention to less salient details is required. 相似文献