全文获取类型
收费全文 | 215篇 |
免费 | 59篇 |
国内免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
284篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 10篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 8篇 |
2013年 | 14篇 |
2012年 | 8篇 |
2011年 | 12篇 |
2010年 | 2篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 9篇 |
2007年 | 11篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 6篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有284条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
基于分部评分模型的思路, 本文提出了一般化的分部评分认知诊断模型(General Partial Credit Diagnostic Model, GPCDM), 与国际上已有的基于分部评分模型思路的多级评分模型GDM (von Davier, 2008 )和PC-DINA (de la Torre, 2012 )相比, GPCDM的Q矩阵定义更加灵活, 项目参数的约束条件更少。Monte Carlo实验研究表明, GPCDM模型的参数估计精度指标RMSE介于[0.015, 0.043], 表明估计精度尚可; TIMSS (2007)实证数据应用研究表明, 与GDM和PC-DINA模型相比, GPCDM与该数据的拟合度更好, 并且使用GPCDM分析该数据的诊断效果也更优。总之, 本研究提供了一种约束条件更少、功能更为强大的多级评分认知诊断模型。 相似文献
62.
基于结构方程模型的多重中介效应分析 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
多重中介模型是指存在多个中介变量的模型。多重中介模型可以分析特定中介效应、总的中介效应和对比中介效应。指出了目前多重中介模型分析普遍存在的问题,包括分析不完整、使用Sobel检验带来的局限。建议通过增加辅助变量的方法进行完整的多重中介效应分析,使用偏差校正的Bootstrap方法进行中介检验。总结出一个多重中介SEM分析流程,并有示例和相应的MPLUS程序。随后展望了辅助变量和中介效应检验方法的发展方向。 相似文献
63.
64.
《心理发展与教育》2025,41(2)
通过遵循PRISMA-Protocol,对42项研究(总样本量为1751)运用元分析技术,以不同认知功能的任务指标为变量来探究午后小睡对认知功能的作用效果及调节因素。结果发现:与控制条件相比,午后小睡对认知功能的作用效果达到中等的正向效应量(g=0.59,k=130,p < 0.001)。亚组分析结果表明,午后小睡的作用效果存在认知领域的特异性和年龄的差异性:午后小睡可以显著提高个体的执行功能、陈述性记忆和程序性记忆的表现,但不影响注意功能;相比于青年群体,午后小睡对未成年和中老年群体认知功能的助益效果更加明显。午后小睡时长、是否有午后小睡习惯、实验日前晚是否存在睡眠限制以及任务后测时间点等因素对午后小睡的效果无显著调节作用。 相似文献
65.
66.
The posterior analysis in estimating factor score in a confirmatory factor analysis model with polytomous, censored or truncated data is investigated in this paper. For the above three types of data, posterior distributions of the factor score are studied, and the estimators of the factor score are obtained to be the location parameters of the posterior distributions. The accuracy of Bayesian estimates is studied via simulation studies.This research was supported by a Hong Kong UGC grant. 相似文献
67.
Matching with a trio of concurrent variable-interval schedules of reinforcement 总被引:5,自引:4,他引:1
下载免费PDF全文

A trio of concurrent variable-interval schedules of reinforcement was arranged according to a changeover-key procedure, including a changeover delay of 1.5 sec. The three schedules provided a combined maximum reinforcement rate of 45 reinforcements per hour. With that restriction, the nine experimental conditions included several combinations of variable-interval schedules, sometimes including extinction. The pigeons matched relative response rate and relative time to relative reinforcement rate. Relative time appeared to match some-what better than relative response rate. Performance adjusted rapidly from one experimental condition to the next, whether the change involved two or all three schedules of the concurrent trio. 相似文献
68.
RESPONSE REDUCTION THROUGH THE SUPERIMPOSITION OF CONTINUOUS REINFORCEMENT: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION
Prior clinical research suggests that superimposition and subsequent removal of a schedule of continuous reinforcement (CRF) may be a viable rate-decreasing procedure in that an extinction-like condition is arranged. The arrangement of similar conditions in the laboratory, however, resulted in the quick recovery of baseline rates. Lever-pressing patterns of eight male rats maintained by different schedules of variable-ratio and variable-interval food reinforcement were examined in an A-B-A experimental design of CRF food superimposition and removal. Responding was substantially reduced during the superimposition of CRF. Upon removal of the superimposed schedule, responding quickly approached presuperimposition baseline rates. 相似文献
69.
Paul R. Rosenbaum 《Psychometrika》1987,52(2):217-233
Test items are often evaluated and compared by contrasting the shapes of their item characteristics curves (ICC's) or surfaces. The current paper develops and applies three general (i.e., nonparametric) comparisons of the shapes of two item characteristic surfaces: (i) proportional latent odds, (ii) uniform relative difficulty, and (iii) item sensitivity. Two items may be compared in these ways while making no assumption about the shapes of item characteristic surfaces for other items, and no assumption about the dimensionality of the latent variable. Also studied is a method for comparing the relative shapes of two item characteristic curves in two examinee populations.The author is grateful to Paul Holland, Robert Mislevy, Tue Tjur, Rebecca Zwick, the editor and reviewers for valuable comments on the subject of this paper, to Mari A. Pearlman for advice on the pairing of items in the examples, and to Dorothy Thayer for assistance with computing. 相似文献
70.
The behavior of individual pigeons on fixed-ratio, variable-ratio, and random-ratio schedules was examined. Within each type of ratio schedule the size of the ratio was varied in an irregular sequence. At various ratio sizes (5, 10, 40, 80) no differences were found among overall response rates (postreinforcement pause plus running response rate) as a function of ratio type. This similarity in overall response rates held despite noticeable differences in the microstructure of performance both within and across subjects; the primary performance difference on the three types of ratio schedules was the relatively longer postreinforcement pause duration on the fixed-ratio schedule. We concluded that the gross temporal characteristics of performance determined by the relative weightings of the postreinforcement pause and running response rate were primarily controlled by the type of ratio schedule (fixed, variable, or random), whereas the overall rate of responding was controlled by the size of the ratio. 相似文献