全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5460篇 |
免费 | 338篇 |
国内免费 | 129篇 |
专业分类
5927篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 9篇 |
2024年 | 122篇 |
2023年 | 168篇 |
2022年 | 75篇 |
2021年 | 179篇 |
2020年 | 308篇 |
2019年 | 317篇 |
2018年 | 209篇 |
2017年 | 264篇 |
2016年 | 246篇 |
2015年 | 215篇 |
2014年 | 225篇 |
2013年 | 714篇 |
2012年 | 167篇 |
2011年 | 239篇 |
2010年 | 166篇 |
2009年 | 235篇 |
2008年 | 249篇 |
2007年 | 256篇 |
2006年 | 239篇 |
2005年 | 211篇 |
2004年 | 202篇 |
2003年 | 167篇 |
2002年 | 156篇 |
2001年 | 92篇 |
2000年 | 87篇 |
1999年 | 62篇 |
1998年 | 57篇 |
1997年 | 40篇 |
1996年 | 52篇 |
1995年 | 46篇 |
1994年 | 39篇 |
1993年 | 26篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有5927条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
Infants' understanding of toy model-real exemplar relations was assessed through preferential looking and habituation tasks. Results from the preferential looking task suggest that 18-month toddlers are just beginning to demonstrate comprehension of symbolic relations between iconic models and their real object counterparts. Performance of 10- and 14-month-old infants in the preferential looking task did not improve when across-domain pairs of videos were used in place of within-domain pairs. Habituation task results indicated that 10-month-olds do not comprehend symbolic relations between miniature toy replicas and their "real" counterparts, but that such understanding begins to emerge by age 14 months. Interactions between symbolic processing and early lexical development are considered, as are methodological implications for the study of infant categorization. 相似文献
152.
Lichtenberg J 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2004,49(2):133-142
This paper begins with a view of the remarkable understanding of infant and child development that has evolved from research and observation. The limitations of this contribution from science to the multi-dimensional context-based individuality of each human in his or her intersubjective realm are then considered. For a contemporary view we must recognize the influence of the variability of experiences and the inferences drawn from them. Inferences involve symbolization and culturally derived archetypes as illustrated in a clinical example. 相似文献
153.
Learning about life and death in early childhood 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
have argued that young children initially understand biological phenomena in terms of vitalism, a mode of construal in which "life" or "life-force" is the central causal-explanatory concept. This study investigated the development of vitalistic reasoning in young children's concepts of life, the human body and death. Sixty preschool children between the ages of 3 years, 7 months and 5 years, 11 months participated. All children were initially given structured interviews to assess their knowledge of (1) human body function and (2) death. From this sample 40 children in the Training group were taught about the human body and how it functions to maintain life. The Control group (n=20) received no training. All 60 children were subsequently reassessed on their knowledge of human body function and death. Results from the initial interviews indicated that young children who spontaneously appealed to vitalistic concepts in reasoning about human body functioning were also more sophisticated in their understanding of death. Results from the posttraining interviews showed that children readily learned to adopt a vitalistic approach to human body functioning, and that this learning coincided with significant development in their understanding of human body function, and of death. The overall pattern of results supports the claim that the acquisition of a vitalistic causal-explanatory framework serves to structure children's concepts and facilitates learning in the domain of biology. 相似文献
154.
Two studies were conducted to examine developmental progression in children's and adults' ability to adequately monitor their own attempts to recall event details as well as the dependence of such metamemorial competencies on question formats. Eight and 10-year-old children as well as adults (Study 1, N=116; Study 2, N=60) rated their confidence when responding to specific questions about an observed event. Confirming most recent results, children and adults gave higher confidence ratings after correct than after incorrect answers. This ability, however, was limited to an unbiased question format. When being asked misleading questions, children's ability to differentiate was undermined, as reflected in equally high confidence judgments after correct and incorrect answers, even when the interview contained a mix of misleading and unbiased questions. When the interviewer "bombarded" the children with an uninterrupted series of misleading questions, children's difficulties appeared to be even more pronounced. These findings highlight the importance of the way in which questions are asked, and point to age-related progression in the relative impact of questioning style. 相似文献
155.
This article reports on the development and teaching of compulsory courses on ethics and engineering at Delft University of
Technology (DUT). Attention is paid to the teaching goals, the educational setup and methods, the contents of the courses,
involvement of staff from engineering schools, experiences to date, and challenges for the future. The choices made with respect
to the development and teaching of the courses are placed within the European and Dutch context and are compared and contrasted
with the American situation and experiences.
Previous versions of this article were presented at the meeting of the SEFI Working Group on Engineering Ethics in Rzeszow,
Poland, April 29–May 1, 1999 and at the SEFI Annual Conference in Zurich, Switzerland, September 1–3, 1999. 相似文献
156.
This review summarizes the literature on the relationship between marital conflict and child maladjustment with an emphasis on variables that qualify, explain the association, or both. Following a historical review, the modest findings on the strength of the association between marital conflict and child maladjustment is explored. The definition of marital conflict is clarified through specification of its various dimensions (frequency, intensity, content, resolution). The role of variables that serve to moderate and/or mediate the relationship between marital conflict and child maladjustment are elaborated. Mediating models include exposure theories (Modeling, Cognitive–Contextual effects: appraisal of threat and blame, and Emotional Insecurity) and changes in the parent–child relationship (Spillover). Variables that moderate or qualify the relationship include children's cognitions and behaviors, contextual factors, and demographic differences. A model is presented summarizing these mechanisms. Research recommendations are proposed and the clinical implications of this literature are addressed. 相似文献
157.
James E. Wanek 《International Journal of Selection & Assessment》1999,7(4):183-195
This selective review of integrity and honesty testing addresses two primary questions: ‘What do we know about honesty testing?’ and ‘How do we use what we know?’ Up-to-date information about test reliability, validity, and construct definition from recent reviews of the research literature in the USA is presented and interpreted. Relationships to other selection devices and personality measures are discussed, as well as how integrity tests fit into a multiple assessment selection system. 相似文献
158.
Rolf Loeber Ph.D. Stephanie M. Green M.A. Benjamin B. Lahey Ph.D. Mary Anne G. Christ M.A. Paul J. Frick Ph.D. 《Journal of child and family studies》1992,1(1):21-41
Retrospective and prospective reports of the onset of disruptive child behaviors were analyzed in a sample of clinic-referred boys. The younger boys (ages 7–9 years), compared with the older boys (ages 10–12 years), showed the highest level of disruptive behavior and, judging from mothers' reports, had the fastest progression of onsets from less serious to more serious problem behaviors. Despite some overlap, developmental sequences in problem behavior within the domains of hyperactivity/inattention, oppositional behavior, and conduct problems were similar across the two age groups. This was also the case for developmental sequences of overt or confrontive problem behaviors and covert or concealing conduct problems. Sequences of the onset of oppositional behavior and conduct problems were validated through prospective data, based on the information from mothers, teachers, and boys themselves. The implications of the findings are discussed for the formulation of developmental pathways of behavior and the analysis of causal factors. 相似文献
159.
以横断研究方法探讨一生发展中搜索速度对短时记忆容量的影响。以记忆广度任务和斯腾伯格任务分别测量6~70岁被试短时记忆容量与记忆搜索速度。结果发现:记忆广度在16岁达到最高峰,以后开始下降;一生的不同阶段,斯腾伯格任务中成功搜索的加工过程基本一致,但搜索速度一直在发展;搜索速度可能是贯穿短时记忆发展一生的影响因素。 相似文献
160.
民族道德与社会政治道德具有不可分割的关系。一般说来,民族道德易受制于政治道德,而民族道德又会反作用于政治道德,同时,民族道德与政治道德作为一种有联系的精神实体,又会在相互影响、相互作用的过程中协调发展。 相似文献