全文获取类型
收费全文 | 932篇 |
免费 | 105篇 |
国内免费 | 46篇 |
专业分类
1083篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 2篇 |
2024年 | 23篇 |
2023年 | 39篇 |
2022年 | 25篇 |
2021年 | 36篇 |
2020年 | 68篇 |
2019年 | 72篇 |
2018年 | 49篇 |
2017年 | 75篇 |
2016年 | 48篇 |
2015年 | 29篇 |
2014年 | 38篇 |
2013年 | 148篇 |
2012年 | 31篇 |
2011年 | 27篇 |
2010年 | 12篇 |
2009年 | 32篇 |
2008年 | 22篇 |
2007年 | 37篇 |
2006年 | 42篇 |
2005年 | 36篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 30篇 |
2002年 | 20篇 |
2001年 | 18篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有1083条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
851.
In an attempt to increase the reliability of the demonstrator-consistent responding effect produced in the bidirectional
control procedure, experiments 1–4 sought conditions that would magnify the matching effect. The aim was to produce a robust
demonstrator-consistent responding effect in order that future analytic experiments could investigate the psychological processes
responsible for this effect. The joystick responses of observer rats trained using the standard bidirectional control procedure
parameters were compared with those of observers subject to conditions identified in the social learning literature as favourable
for imitation. Unlike mice, observer rats in experiments 1 a and 1 b tended to push a joystick in the same direction as their
demonstrators when the demonstrators were either familiar or unfamiliar males and females. Comparable demonstrator-consistent
responding occurred following observation of a standard and a salient joystick response (experiment 2). Experiment 3 showed
that the discriminative accuracy of a demonstrator’s responding was important for matching behaviour, and suggested that matching
might be enhanced with more than the conventional single observation session. Experiment 4 confirmed that the bidirectional
control effect is sensitive to the amount of observational experience; after six observation sessions, demonstrator-inconsistent
responding occurs. The results of experiments 1–3 are, and those of experiment 4 are not, compatible with the hypothesis that
demonstrator-consistent responding in the bidirectional control is caused by olfactory cues deposited by demonstrators on
the joystick.
Received: 29 May 2000 / Accepted after revision: 28 August 2000 相似文献
852.
‘What's New?’ at the Dinner Table: Family Dynamics During Mealtimes in Two Cultural Groups in Hawaii
Mary Martini 《Infant and child development》1996,5(1):23-34
This study shows systematic differences in how parents structure mealtime discourse in two cultural groups in Hawaii. Fifty-nine families were videotaped during week-day evening meals at home. Thirty of the families were middle-class Japanese-American (JA) families; 29 were middle-class Caucasian-American (CA) families. Evening meals in the Caucasian-American homes in this sample are clear-cut events. Families sit at tables and talk throughout the meal. Children are encouraged to describe their experiences, wishes, plans and perceptions. Parents cue children to focus on distinctive aspects of their realities, to talk about ‘what's new’, or unfamiliar to the audience. Evening meals in the Japanese-American homes are less focused on formal discussion. Half the families watch television while eating; others play games, cook, listen to music, talk on the phone or play with babies or pets during dinner. JA family members talk to each other less and structure their discourse differently. Rather than report on distinct, individual experiences, JA family members co-narrate and comment on what they are collectively doing. The observed differences are related to cultural differences in construals of self and other. The cultural importance of being able to clearly communicate individual realities across separate beings is seen in the CA mealtimes. Appreciation of smooth, group process and coordinated belonging is seen in the JA evening meals. 相似文献
853.
854.
Nicole Lucassen Rianne Kok Marian J. Bakermans‐Kranenburg Marinus H. Van Ijzendoorn Vincent W. V. Jaddoe Albert Hofman Frank C. Verhulst Mijke P. Lambregtse‐Van den Berg Henning Tiemeier 《The British journal of developmental psychology》2015,33(4):489-505
We investigated the association between mothers' and fathers' harsh parenting and sensitive parenting practices and child's executive functions (EF) in early childhood in 607 families. We focused on three broad dimensions of child EF: Emergent metacognition, inhibitory self‐control, and flexibility measured with the parent‐reported Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function‐Preschool Version. Less sensitive parenting of the mother and harsher parenting of the father were related to lower scores of emergent metacognition and inhibitory self‐control. Parenting was not associated with child flexibility. This study extends previous research on the association between parenting and EF by the focus on the role of the father and demonstrates independent effects of mother and father on child EF. 相似文献
855.
Xiaopeng Gong Kathryn L. Fletcher Jocelyn H. Bolin 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2015,93(3):259-268
This study explored the relationships among parenting styles, perfectionism dimensions, and coping through an online survey. With structural equation modeling and a bootstrap method, the results demonstrated that perfectionism dimensions mediated the relationship between parenting styles and coping strategies among 376 undergraduates (women, 80.6%; men, 19.4%; Caucasian, 94.4%). Thus, parenting practices may be antecedents to perfectionism dimensions that contribute to an individual's ability to cope with stressful situations. The implications for counselors and practice are discussed. 相似文献
856.
Philipp Jugert Katharina Eckstein Andreas Beelmann Peter Noack 《European Journal of Developmental Psychology》2016,13(2):213-230
This study investigated to what extent parents’ intergroup attitudes influence the development of their children’s ethnic intergroup attitudes (i.e., ingroup bias), and which socialization factors moderate this influence. We report on a four-year longitudinal study of 213 children (Mage = 7.94) and their parents conducted in Eastern Germany. Findings showed significant interindividual differences in children’s developmental trajectories, which could, in part, be explained by child gender, socio-economic status and parental attitudes. The influence of parents’ intergroup attitudes was moderated by parenting style and parental similarity in attitudes both of which, our findings suggest, can make the transmission of parental attitudes more likely. 相似文献
857.
Frouke Hermens Robin Walker 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2016,107(3):484-502
Research has shown that social and symbolic cues presented in isolation and at fixation have strong effects on observers, but it is unclear how cues compare when they are presented away from fixation and embedded in natural scenes. We here compare the effects of two types of social cue (gaze and pointing gestures) and one type of symbolic cue (arrow signs) on eye movements of observers under two viewing conditions (free viewing vs. a memory task). The results suggest that social cues are looked at more quickly, for longer and more frequently than the symbolic arrow cues. An analysis of saccades initiated from the cue suggests that the pointing cue leads to stronger cueing than the gaze and the arrow cue. While the task had only a weak influence on gaze orienting to the cues, stronger cue following was found for free viewing compared to the memory task. 相似文献
858.
859.
860.
Loren Vandenbroucke Karine Verschueren Eva Ceulemans Bert De Smedt Kim De Roover Dieter Baeyens 《The British journal of developmental psychology》2016,34(2):226-244
Executive functioning (EF), needed for goal‐oriented behaviour, thoughts, and emotions, is important for various life domains. This study examined the relationship between family demographics and EF subcomponents. A kindergarten sample was tested on subcomponents of working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility. Parents provided information on demographic variables. For 78 children both EF and demographic data were available. First, demographic profiles were identified within the sample. Two profiles were found: A low‐risk profile of mainly two‐biological‐parent, high‐income families with a highly educated mother who did not smoke during pregnancy and a high‐risk profile of low‐income families with a young, low‐educated mother who more often smoked during pregnancy. Second, children with different demographic profiles were compared on EF subcomponents. Results indicate differential relations between family demographics and EF subcomponents: Whereas for most EF subcomponents no association with family demographics was found, high‐risk children performed better on response shifting and tended to perform worse on verbal memory than low‐risk children. Parenting stress decreased performance only for high‐risk children. Although this study found limited impact of family demographics for EF, further longitudinal research can provide nuanced insights about which factors influence specific EF subcomponents during which developmental periods and guide targeted prevention of EF difficulties. 相似文献