全文获取类型
收费全文 | 571篇 |
免费 | 63篇 |
国内免费 | 21篇 |
专业分类
655篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 4篇 |
2024年 | 11篇 |
2023年 | 22篇 |
2022年 | 18篇 |
2021年 | 30篇 |
2020年 | 42篇 |
2019年 | 36篇 |
2018年 | 33篇 |
2017年 | 45篇 |
2016年 | 32篇 |
2015年 | 20篇 |
2014年 | 16篇 |
2013年 | 92篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 5篇 |
2009年 | 15篇 |
2008年 | 13篇 |
2007年 | 16篇 |
2006年 | 27篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 22篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有655条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
121.
大学生网络成瘾倾向与父母教养方式关系研究 总被引:56,自引:0,他引:56
本文使用父母教养方式评价量表(EMBU)和网络成瘾倾向自陈量表,对陕西四所高校大学生进行调查,结果表明,网络成瘾倾向者与非网络成瘾倾向者父母教养方式差异较为显著,表现在过分干涉、惩罚严厉、拒绝否定等方面。 相似文献
122.
Stefan Artmann 《Axiomathes》2004,14(4):381-395
The French molecular biologist François Jacob outlined a theory of evolution as tinkering. From a methodological point of view, his approach can be seen as a biologic specification of the relation between laws, describing coherently the dynamics of a system, and contingent boundary conditions on this dynamics. From a semiotic perspective, tinkering is a pragmatic concept well-known from the information-theoretic anthropology of Claude Lévi-Strauss. In idealized contrast to an engineer, the tinkerer has to accept the concrete restrictions on his material resources as only gradually changeable constraints on his projects. Jacobian biopragmatics examines evolution as a biologic analogue to human tinkering devoid of any projecting subjectivity. To validate this analogy, four basic principles concerning main formal aspects of evolutionary objects, agents, histories, and consistency criteria are proposed.This revised version was published online in October 2005 with corrections to the Cover Date. 相似文献
123.
Meredith-Owen W 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2008,53(4):459-480
This paper is concerned with the operation of envy: it considers its origins and their repercussions, in particular its compulsion to sterilize the evidence of fertility, exemplified by Roger Money-Kyrle's evocation of 'the parental intercourse as the supremely creative act'. The inevitable impact on the analytic relationship is considered in the context of two clinical examples of impasse. It is argued that such fundamentally negative transferences, one full of overt aggression and enactment, the other more covertly sabotaging, derive from envious retaliatory impulses originating in experiences--whether phantasied or factual--of exclusion from the anticipated 'good'. Significant recent Jungian contributions to this area are considered and the absence of references to 'envy'--so characteristic of Kleinian discourse--is noted. The ongoing value of integrating Jungian and Kleinian approaches is affirmed. 相似文献
124.
This paper makes a point about the interpretation of the simplestquantified modal logic, that is, quantified modal logic witha single domain. It is commonly assumed that the domain in questionis to be understood as the set of all possibile objects. Thepoint of the paper is that this assumption is misguided. 相似文献
125.
Sarah M. Shuwairi 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2009,104(1):115-123
Can infants use interposition and line junction cues to infer three-dimensional (3D) structure? Previous work has shown that in a task that required 4-month-olds to discriminate between static two-dimensional (2D) pictures of possible and impossible cubes, infants exhibited a spontaneous preference for displays of the impossible cube but left open the question of whether they did so on the basis of purely local “critical regions” or whether they were able to employ more global clues. Here infants were presented with possible and impossible cubes in which the strictly local cues that could have derived from exterior binding contours were deleted. Results showed that infants were still able to discriminate possible cubes from impossible cubes, suggesting that longer looking infants are sensitive to global properties and that the capacity to integrate pictorial information to perceive aspects of global 3D shape may develop earlier than demonstrated previously using reaching tasks. 相似文献
126.
127.
Alberto Voltolini 《Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences》2009,8(1):135-150
In his (2001a) and in some related papers, Tim Crane has maintained that intentional objects are schematic entities, in the sense that, insofar as being an intentional object is not a genuine metaphysical category, qua objects of thought intentional objects have no particular nature. This approach to intentionalia is the metaphysical counterpart of the later Husserl's ontological approach to the same entities, according to which qua objects of thought intentionalia are indifferent to existence. But to buy a metaphysically deflationary approach does not mean to buy an ontologically deflationary approach, according to which we have to accept all the intentional objects there apparently are. Being metaphysically deflationary on intentionalia rather means that from the ontological point of view one must really allow only for those intentionalia for which one is entitled to say that there are such things; typically, for which an ontological proof is available. From metaphysical schematism plus conditional, or partial, ontological committment to intentionalia, further interesting consequences follow. First, this theoretical combination allows one to deal with the ‘too-many entities’ problem (may one fail to accept an ontological proof for an entity of a given kind if she thinks that the entity we would have to be committed to is an entity of another kind?). Second, it allows one to deal with the ‘genuinely true report’ problem (how is it that if we exercise mindreading with respect to a somehow deluded person, we want our reports to come out as really, not merely fictionally, true?). 相似文献
128.
Xianglong Zhang 《Frontiers of Philosophy in China》2007,2(3):309-335
The reason for the emergence of consciousness of filial piety is that parental care could activate reciprocal filial piety.
Parental care and filial piety are two supplementary phenomena caused by the same time consciousness. Phenomenology neglects
consciousness of filial piety because it lacks the thinking that sees the fundamental “meaning of time” in the intersection
of “past” and “future”. The consciousness of filial piety can only be really constituted by a human being’s personal experience.
“Frustrations in personal life” and “breeding of children for oneself” are two occasions for an adult to fight against the
separating effect of individualized consciousness and regain awareness of filial piety.
Translated by Huang Deyuan from Beijing Daxue Xuebao 北京大学学报 (Journal of Peking University), 2006, (1): 14–24 相似文献
129.
130.
为探讨父母学业参与和学业压力与青少年早期学业投入之间的关系,考察成就目标取向的中介作用,以及学业成就水平在该中介过程中的调节作用,对2487名初一学生进行测查,结果发现:(1)父母学业参与正向预测青少年早期个体的学业投入、负向预测学业抽离,父母学业压力正向预测学业抽离;(2)掌握目标取向在父母学业参与和青少年早期的学业投入之间起正向中介作用,在父母学业参与和学业抽离间发挥负向中介作用;表现回避目标取向在父母学业压力和学业抽离间具有正向中介作用;(3)掌握目标取向的中介作用受到学业成就水平的调节。研究结果揭示了父母学业参与和学业压力对青少年早期的学业投入的共同作用,为从家庭层面提高青少年的学业投入水平提供了参考。 相似文献