全文获取类型
收费全文 | 734篇 |
免费 | 76篇 |
国内免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
819篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 24篇 |
2020年 | 25篇 |
2019年 | 27篇 |
2018年 | 29篇 |
2017年 | 36篇 |
2016年 | 22篇 |
2015年 | 28篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 102篇 |
2012年 | 26篇 |
2011年 | 21篇 |
2010年 | 18篇 |
2009年 | 22篇 |
2008年 | 39篇 |
2007年 | 36篇 |
2006年 | 42篇 |
2005年 | 29篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 21篇 |
2002年 | 19篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 14篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有819条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
201.
The transfer of specific and general consequential functions through simple and conditional equivalence relations
下载免费PDF全文

Hayes SC Kohlenberg BS Hayes LJ 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1991,56(1):119-137
The purpose of this study was to examine the transfer of consequential (reinforcement and punishment) functions through equivalence relations. In Experiment 1, 9 subjects acquired three three-member equivalence classes through matching-to-sample training using arbitrary visual forms. Comparison stimuli were then given conditioned reinforcement or punishment functions by pairing them with verbal feedback during a sorting task. For 8 of the 9 subjects, trained consequential functions transferred through their respective equivalence classes without additional training. In Experiment 2, transfer of function was initially tested before equivalence testing per se. Three of 4 subjects showed the transfer without a formal equivalence test. In Experiment 3, 3 subjects were given training that gave rise to six new three-member conditional equivalence classes. For 2 of the subjects, the same stimulus could have either a reinforcement or punishment function on the basis of contextual cues that defined its class membership. Experiment 4 assessed whether equivalence training had established general or specific consequential functions primarily by adding novel stimuli in the transfer test. Subjects treated even novel feedback stimuli in the transfer test as consequences, but the direction of consequential effects depended upon the transfer of specific consequential functions through equivalence relations. 相似文献
202.
203.
An interdependent group-oriented contingency and graphic feedback were used to increase the activity levels of residents of a group home for persons with traumatic brain injury. Results showed that the intervention was effective for 4 of the 6 subjects. Individual performances must be examined when implementing group contingencies because all subjects may not respond. 相似文献
204.
Linda Jarchow Jones 《Zygon》1994,29(1):115-125
Abstract. In this paper, I explore, as a Christian and a parish pastor, what drew me into the religion-science dialogue and what keeps me involved. Encounters with nature and readings of evolutionary theory answer some questions and raise others, especially questions about chance and the nature of God. I persist in my quest for understanding because creedal affirmations of God as Creator demand an examination of the relationship between God and the world, and because I want to proclaim the Christian message in a credible way to parishioners raised with a scientific worldview. Along the way I am reaping unexpected spiritual dividends. 相似文献
205.
Contributions to factor analysis of dichotomous variables 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Bengt Muthén 《Psychometrika》1978,43(4):551-560
A new method is proposed for the factor analysis of dichotomous variables. Similar to the method of Christoffersson this uses information from the first and second order proportions to fit a multiple factor model. Through a transformation into a new set of sample characteristics, the estimation is considerably simplified. A generalized least-squares estimator is proposed, which asymptotically is as efficient as the corresponding estimator of Christoffersson, but which demands less computing time.This research was supported by the Bank of Sweden Tercentenary Foundation under project Structural Equation Models in the Social Sciences, project director Karl G. Jöreskog. 相似文献
206.
Neumann JK 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1977,10(4):755-758
The data analyses utilized in group contingency projects are reviewed. Previous studies are cited to emphasize advantages of nonconsolidated ("individual") over consolidated analyses. Several procedures are described that enable applied researchers to incorporate nonconsolidated data analyses in group contingency studies. 相似文献
207.
Transient effects of acquisition history on generalization in a matching-to-sample task 总被引:3,自引:3,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

This study examined the role of learning history on the acquisition of a matching-to-sample task. Twelve preschool children learned four stimulus classes through instructions, shaping, or imitation. After reaching criterion, the subjects were exposed to changed discrimination contingencies to determine how each learning history affected the acquisition of responses appropriate to the new contingencies. All subjects reached criterion on the new relations, although the subjects with a shaping history adapted slightly more quickly than those subjects with a history of instructions or imitation. Given sufficient exposure to changed contingencies, rule-driven insensitivity to contingencies was overcome by experience with consequences. This result may be specific to younger subjects, but it suggests that instructions can be used in education without creating insensitivity to contingencies. 相似文献
208.
209.
Sunita S. Ahlawat 《决策行为杂志》1999,12(1):71-88
Sequential processing of evidence may lead to recency effect, a potential bias in judgment. The present research seeks to extend the literature on recency effects by assessing the potential moderating influence of team work: whether group decision making moderates the severity of recency effects predicted by Hogarth and Einhorn (1992), and whether group processing influences the accuracy of, and confidence in memory for evidence. Experienced auditors from a Big‐6 accounting firm made audit judgments, either individually or as groups. They were randomly assigned to one of two levels of evidence presentation order. After performing the judgment task, participants completed two evidence recognition tests. Consistent with prior findings, recency effects on judgments were observed, but only for individuals. Group judgments or audit reports were not affected by recency. Order effects, however, did not translate into different choices of audit reports, and did not persist in memories of either individuals or groups. As expected, group memory was more accurate than individual memory and groups were more confident than individuals. Overall, confidence in accurate memories was greater than in inaccurate ones. Copyright © 1999 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
210.
A self-reflecting signed order is a preference relation that compares relative likes and dislikes for items in a set X by jointly ordering X and a disjoint copy of X. If you would like Jones but not Smith appointed to a committee, and also think it more important to exclude Smith than include Jones, your self-reflecting signed order records this information. We review basic representational theory for signed orders, then examine them when X is multiattributed. Axioms for additive measurement of multiattribute self-reflecting signed orders are specified for several X-structures. 相似文献