全文获取类型
收费全文 | 219篇 |
免费 | 134篇 |
国内免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
361篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 24篇 |
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 17篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 5篇 |
2015年 | 15篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 18篇 |
2012年 | 5篇 |
2011年 | 15篇 |
2010年 | 15篇 |
2009年 | 15篇 |
2008年 | 19篇 |
2007年 | 8篇 |
2006年 | 9篇 |
2005年 | 10篇 |
2004年 | 11篇 |
2003年 | 5篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有361条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
281.
Establishing derived categorical responding in children with disabilities using the PEAK‐E curriculum 下载免费PDF全文
Mark R. Dixon Jordan Belisle Caleb R. Stanley Ryan C. Speelman Kyle E. Rowsey Dena Kime Jacob H. Daar 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2017,50(1):134-145
The purpose of the study was to evaluate a procedure to generate derived categorical responding by three children with disabilities and to promote the emergence of untrained intraverbal categorical responses. In the study, three 4‐member equivalence classes including three stimuli (A, B, and C) and a category name (D) for each class were trained using a match‐to‐sample procedure. Test probes were conducted for categorical responding, including both a trained (D‐A) and two derived (D‐B, D‐C) relational responses, as well as the emergence of untrained intraverbal categorical responding (D‐A/B/C) throughout the study. Relational training was effective at promoting the emergence of categorical responding, and two of the three participants demonstrated the emergence of additional intraverbal responding without prior training. The results provide further evidence supporting the practical utility of stimulus equivalence as well as the PEAK‐E curriculum. 相似文献
282.
Sebastian Sequoiah-Grayson 《Journal of Philosophical Logic》2008,37(1):67-94
This article provides the first comprehensive reconstruction and analysis of Hintikka’s attempt to obtain a measure of the
information yield of deductive inferences. The reconstruction is detailed by necessity due to the originality of Hintikka’s
contribution. The analysis will turn out to be destructive. It dismisses Hintikka’s distinction between surface information and depth information as being of any utility towards obtaining a measure of the information yield of deductive inferences. Hintikka is right to
identify the failure of canonical information theory to give an account of the information yield of deductions as a scandal,
however this article demonstrates that his attempt to provide such an account fails. It fails primarily because it applies
to only a restricted set of deductions in the polyadic predicate calculus, and fails to apply at all to the deductions in
the monadic predicate calculus and the propositional calculus. Some corollaries of these facts are a number of undesirable
and counterintuitive results concerning the proposed relation of linguistic meaning (and hence synonymy) with surface information.
Some of these results will be seen to contradict Hintikka’s stated aims, whilst others are seen to be false. The consequence
is that the problem of obtaining a measure of the information yield of deductive inferences remains an open one. The failure
of Hintikka’s proposal will suggest that a purely syntactic approach to the problem be abandoned in favour of an intrinsically
semantic one. 相似文献
283.
Young children’s ability to learn something new from a third-party interaction may be related to the ability to imagine themselves in the third-party interaction. This imaginative ability presupposes an understanding of self-other equivalence, which is manifested in an objective understanding of the self and an understanding of others’ subjective perspectives. The current study measured imitative learning of a novel action seen only in a third-party interaction, mirror self-recognition, and perspective taking in a group of 48 18- to 20-month-olds. Patterns of performance suggest that understanding self-other equivalence is related to third-party learning. 相似文献
284.
Miguel CF Petursdottir AI Carr JE Michael J 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2008,89(3):383-405
The purpose of the current study was to assess whether children would categorize pictures when taught the relevant listener and speaker behaviors separately. A category-sort test was used to assess emergent conditional relations. Category-sort trials consisted of looking at (Test 1) or tacting/labeling (Test 2) a sample stimulus and selecting the appropriate comparison stimuli. In Experiment 1, 4 children (3-5 years) were taught to tact pictures of six U.S. state maps as either north or south. An assessment was conducted to determine whether they would (1) correctly categorize or sort when presented with a visual sample and (2) select the correct stimuli when hearing their category names (listener behavior). Two of the children categorized the pictures during Posttest 1 after the initial (pairwise) tact training. The other 2 categorized after receiving additional tact training with all pictures presented together. However, one of them categorized only during Posttest 2. In Experiment 2, 4 children (3-5 years) were taught to select pictures when hearing their category names. An assessment was conducted to determine whether they would (1) correctly categorize or sort and (2) tact the stimuli (speaker behavior). One child categorized the pictures during Posttest 1, and two during Posttest 2. The other child required additional training with all pictures grouped together. When participants failed to categorize, they also failed to tact the pictures accurately. Taken together, results from Experiments 1 and 2 show that both speaker and listener behavior play an important role in stimulus categorization. 相似文献
285.
Jiri Benovsky 《Philosophical Studies》2008,141(2):175-190
In this paper, I explore several versions of the bundle theory and the substratum theory and compare them, with the surprising
result that it seems to be true that they are equivalent (in a sense of ‘equivalent’ to be specified). In order to see whether
this is correct or not, I go through several steps: first, I examine different versions of the bundle theory with tropes and
compare them to the substratum theory with tropes by going through various standard objections and arguing for a tu quoque in all cases. Emphasizing the theoretical role of the substratum and of the relation of compresence, I defend the claim that
these views are equivalent for all theoretical purposes. I then examine two different versions of the bundle theory with universals,
and show that one of them is, here again, equivalent to the substratum theory with universals, by examining how both views
face the famous objection from Identity of Indiscernibles in a completely parallel way. It is only the second, quite extreme
and puzzling, version of the bundle theory with universals that is not equivalent to any other view; and the diagnosis of
why this is so will show just how unpalatable the view is. Similarly, only a not-so-palatable version of the substratum theory
is genuinely different from the other views; and here again it’s precisely what makes it different that makes it less appealing.
相似文献
Jiri BenovskyEmail: |
286.
Robert A. Brown 《Asian Journal of Social Psychology》2008,11(4):300-304
Most studies of Japanese self‐esteem have implicitly assumed that the concept of self‐esteem (SE) is indigenously meaningful. However, this has not been shown to be true. The present research investigated whether the indigenous Japanese word jisonshin is semantically equivalent to the English expression SE and, by implication, whether it denotes the same concept as originally delineated by Rosenberg, that of having a positive attitude toward oneself. Japanese participants (N = 234) responded to 11 pairs of statements about SE and several other items. Half of the statements contained the expression serufuesutei‐mu (self‐esteem), the other half the expression jisonshin. Participants independently judged the two expressions to be ‘basically’ synonymous, but endorsed some otherwise identical statements significantly more often when they contained the expression serufuesutei‐mu, suggesting that the differences were due to the foreign connotations of the expression serufuesutei‐mu. 相似文献
287.
Sidman M 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2007,87(2):309-316
When we publish behavioral research, we are not allowed to communicate the thrill, the poetry, or the exhilaration that are outcomes of the discovery process. Yet, these are among our most potent reinforcers. Explicit recognition of the emotional accompaniments to research could help attract students into the experimental analysis of behavior. 相似文献
288.
New knowledge derived from learned knowledge: functional-anatomic correlates of stimulus equivalence
Schlund MW Hoehn-Saric R Cataldo MF 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2007,87(2):287-307
Forming new knowledge based on knowledge established through prior learning is a central feature of higher cognition that is captured in research on stimulus equivalence (SE). Numerous SE investigations show that reinforcing behavior under control of distinct sets of arbitrary conditional relations gives rise to stimulus control by new, derived relations. This investigation examined whether frontal-subcortical and frontal-parietal networks known to support reinforced conditional relations also support derived conditional relations. Twelve adult subjects completed matching-to-sample (MTS) training with correct/wrong feedback to establish four trained conditional relations within two distinct, three-member stimulus classes: (1) A1-->B1, B1-->C1 and (2) A2-->B2, B2-->C2. Afterwards, functional neuroimaging was performed when MTS trials were presented involving matching two identical circles (a sensorimotor control condition), trained relations (A-->B, B-->C), and derived relations: symmetry (B-->A, C-->B), transitivity (A-->C), and equivalence (C-->A). Conditional responding to trained and derived relations was similarly correlated with bilateral activation in the targeted networks. Comparing trained to derived relations, however, highlighted greater activation in several prefrontal regions, the caudate, thalamus, and putamen, which may represent the effects of extended training or feedback present during imaging. Each derived relation also evidenced a unique activation pattern. Collectively, the findings extend the role of frontal-subcortical and frontal-parietal networks to derived conditional relations and suggest that regional involvement varies with the type of derived conditional relation. 相似文献
289.
Perkins DR Dougher MJ Greenway DE 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2007,88(1):87-102
This study investigated conditions leading to contextual control by stimulus topography over transfer of functions. Three 4-member stimulus equivalence classes, each consisting of four (A, B, C, D) topographically distinct visual stimuli, were established for 5 college students. Across classes, designated A stimuli were open-ended linear figures, B stimuli were circular, C stimuli three-sided, and D stimuli four-sided. Three different computer tasks then were trained with the B stimuli. Differential reinforcement and punishment procedures were then used to establish control over function transfer by the topography of the class members. For Task 1, function transfer, responding to C and D stimuli as subjects had to B stimuli, was reinforced. For Task 2, function transfer was reinforced for C stimuli but punished for D stimuli. For Task 3, function transfer was punished for both C and D stimuli. New equivalence classes were then established and tests for generalized contextual control were presented. All 5 subjects showed generalized contextual control of transfer of functions by stimulus topography. Implications of contextual control over function transfer in natural settings are discussed. 相似文献
290.
Maria Clara Cordeiro Tatiana Zhirnova Caio F. Miguel 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2021,115(1):340-360
We investigated the role of verbal behavior on the emergence of analogy-type responding as measured via equivalence–equivalence relations. In Experiment 1, 8 college students learned to label arbitrary stimuli as, “vek,” “zog,” and “paf”, and in Experiment 2, 8 additional participants learned to select these stimuli when hearing their names in an auditory–visual matching-to-sample (MTS) task. Experimenters tested for the emergence of relational tacts (i.e., “same” and “different”) and equivalence–equivalence relations (analogy tests) via visual–visual MTS. Half of the participants were exposed to a think-aloud procedure. Even though they all passed analogy tests while tacting stimuli relationally, only participants exposed to tact training (Experiment 1) did so without the need for remediation. The results of these experiments confirm that individual discriminative and relational control of stimuli established through verbal behavior training is sufficient to produce equivalence–equivalence analogical responding, advancing the analysis of complex cognitive (problem-solving) phenomena. 相似文献