全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3881篇 |
免费 | 377篇 |
国内免费 | 146篇 |
专业分类
4404篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 8篇 |
2024年 | 85篇 |
2023年 | 90篇 |
2022年 | 94篇 |
2021年 | 155篇 |
2020年 | 197篇 |
2019年 | 267篇 |
2018年 | 183篇 |
2017年 | 229篇 |
2016年 | 206篇 |
2015年 | 131篇 |
2014年 | 158篇 |
2013年 | 604篇 |
2012年 | 102篇 |
2011年 | 185篇 |
2010年 | 98篇 |
2009年 | 168篇 |
2008年 | 183篇 |
2007年 | 147篇 |
2006年 | 149篇 |
2005年 | 160篇 |
2004年 | 118篇 |
2003年 | 111篇 |
2002年 | 104篇 |
2001年 | 60篇 |
2000年 | 59篇 |
1999年 | 53篇 |
1998年 | 34篇 |
1997年 | 23篇 |
1996年 | 28篇 |
1995年 | 26篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 19篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有4404条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Rats were allowed to forage in a simulated natural environment made up of eight food sources (patches) each containing a fixed number of pellets. Two of the eight contained an extra supply of peanuts. The peanut patches were signaled by an olfactory/visual cue located at the bottom of the ladder leading to the patch. In successive phases the number of sessions per day, height of the patches, and availability of peanuts were manipulated. Subjects showed evidence of discrimination learning under these conditions, although the degree of discriminatory behavior varied as a function of environmental manipulations. Assessment of behavior within foraging sessions showed that subjects systematically changed their patterns of utilization of patches across time. Sampling or exploration, as well as food reinforcement, seem implicated in these results. 相似文献
2.
Vicky V. Wolfe John D. Cone David A. Wolfe 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1986,8(3):211-226
The need to train accurate, not necessarily agreeing, observers is discussed. Intraobserver consistency as an intermediate criterion in such training is proposed and contrasted with the more familiar criterion of interobserver agreement. Videotaped observations of social interactions between handicapped and nonhandicapped preschoolers provided the medium for examining the criterion agreement of four observers trained against each type of standard. Observers generally failed to show high levels of criterion agreement whether trained to a within- or to a between-observer agreement standard. The results varied somewhat with the frequency of behaviors, however. Correlations between interobserver agreement and intraobserver consistency were variable but somewhat higher when interobserver agreement was the training criterion than when intraobserver consistency was the criterion. Correlations between interobserver agreement and criterion agreement ranged from — .16 to .89 during interobserver agreement training. Correlations between intraobserver consistency and criterion agreement ranged from — .23 to .99 during intraobserver consistency training. 相似文献
3.
4.
Edward O. Wilson 《Zygon》1990,25(3):245-262
Abstract. The sciences may be conceptualized as a hierarchy ranked by level of organization (e.g., many-body physics ranks above particle physics). Each science serves as an antidiscipline for the science above it; that is, between each pair, tense but creative interplay is inevitable. Biology has advanced through such tension between its subdisciplines and now can serve as an antidiscipline for the social sciences—for anthropology, for example, by examining the connection between cultural and biological evolution; for psychology, by addressing the nature of learning and the structure of the unconscious; for economics, by examining economically irrational behavior and by comparing economic activity in humans and other species. Sociology, concerned mainly with advanced literate societies, is relatively remote from the genetic basis of human social behavior. However, moving between biological and social levels of organization generates richness and points to new and unexpected principles. 相似文献
5.
Harald Merckelbach J. Leon Kenemans Arie Dijkstra Erik Schouten 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1993,15(3):197-206
A number of experiments have shown that (spider) fearful subjects direct their attention to fear-relevant words, even when these words are irrelevant to the completion of a target task (e.g., color naming). The present study examined whether subjects with an intense fear of spiders also display such attentional bias for a fear-relevant pictoral stimulus. Female spider-fearful (n=13) and control subjects (n=13) saw neutral patterns (i.e., horizontal and vertical bars). One pattern served as target for a reaction time, while the other pattern served as nontarget. Targets and nontargets were accompanied by either fear-relevant or neutral pictoral material (i.e., a picture of a spider or a picture of a flower, respectively). The fear-relevant picture did not selectively delibate reaction time performance of spider-fearful subjects. Thus, no evidence was found for an attentional bias for fear-relevant pictoral material in subjects with an intense fear of spiders. Instead, fearful subjects exhibited a general inhibition of performance which became stronger over trials. This suggests that the fear-relevant picture induced a state of anxious arousal or defensive withdrawal that interfered with reaction time performance on both fear-relevant and neutral trials. 相似文献
6.
Patricia M. Barbetta Ph.D. William L. Heward Ed.D. 《Journal of Behavioral Education》1993,3(3):217-233
An alternating treatments design was used to compare the effects of Active Student Response (ASR) error correction and No Response (NR) error correction during instruction of the capitals of states and countries. Three students with learning disabilities were provided one-to-one daily instruction on four sets of 14 unknown capitals (7 ASR capitals and 7 NR capitals). Student errors during instruction on ASR capitals were immediately followed by the teacher stating the capital and the student repeating it (an active student response). Errors on NR capitals were immediately followed by the teacher stating the capital while the student visually attended to a geography card with the correct capital handwritten on it (an on-task response). During instruction each of the three students correctly stated more capitals taught with ASR instruction than he or she stated with NR error correction. Results of same-day and next-day tests show that all three students learned more capitals with ASR error correction than with NR error correction The students also correctly stated more ASR error correction capitals on 1-week maintenance tests. 相似文献
7.
The mutual exclusivity (ME) assumption states that children affix a novel label to only one unfamiliar object, while the novel-name-nameless category (N3C) assumption states that children affix a novel label to multiple unfamiliar objects. To compare the relative sensitivity of the two assumptions, two types of tasks, with two trials in each, were given to 5-year-olds. In the first trial of each task, all children selected only the unfamiliar object for a novel label, which was consistent with the ME assumption. For the task which did not have the same unfamiliar object in the two trials, 94% of the children selected the object with the same shape but black-white reversed image in the second trial, which was consistent with the N3C assumption. For the task which had the same unfamiliar object in the two trials, 43% of the children selected the same object in the second trial, which was consistent with the ME assumption, and 48% of them selected the object with the same shape but reversed image, which was consistent with the N3C assumption. The findings suggest that the ME and N3C assumptions can be flexibly applied to children's word learning. 相似文献
8.
Mary Addenbrooke 《The Journal of analytical psychology》1997,42(3):507-520
The seminar group can serve as a framework in which to meet the fear generated during training. The group can become a maddening environment, but on occasions when it is experienced by the trainee as good enough, it can act like a playgroup which facilitates development. Experiences in the group can be viewed from the perspectives of group dynamics or of transference phenomena which embody regression. When this regression enables scope for play it can serve in the interests of ego development in the process of discovering the sort of analyst the trainee is becoming as the training unfolds. During play aspects of oneself can merge, then differentiate again. They can have different meanings at the same time. The rational and emotional, the adult and the childlike are all given a place in play. 相似文献
9.
The present paper takes a first step toward the integration of recent findings on brain dynamics and learning into established
fields of psychophysiological science.
Leading-edge studies of brain dynamics have shown that the brain manifests an inherent variability and that, when new stimuli
(i.e., “problems”) are presented to the organism, brain chaos increases. 相似文献
10.
Dubrovskaya Nadezda M. Zhuravin Igor A. 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1995,30(2):127-137
The experiments were performed on adult Wistar male rats trained to push with the forepaw on a fixed piston inside a narrow
tube. It was found that after localized intracerebral injection of a cholinergic antagonist into the dorso-lateral (but not
medial) neostriatum (i.e., the caudato-putamen) the behavioral performance requiring brief innate movements remained unchanged,
but the performance requiring a prolonged pushing movement (> 50 msec) became disrupted. Microinjection of carbacholine (0.03-3
μ g/1 μ1) did not affect the performance of the acquired movements, whereas scopolamine (3 μ g/1 μ1) led to the significant
decrease in pushing time. We conclude that changes in the state of the dorso-lateral neostriatal cholinergic system result
only in disturbances of the sensory-controlled component of a complex instrumental movement.
The 1994 Pavlovian Society Young Investigator Awardee was Nadezda Dubrovskaya, first author of this paper, which was presented
at the annual meeting of the Pavlovian Society, July 3, 1994, in Prague, Czech Republic. 相似文献