全文获取类型
收费全文 | 598篇 |
免费 | 23篇 |
国内免费 | 14篇 |
专业分类
635篇 |
出版年
2025年 | 1篇 |
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 23篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 33篇 |
2020年 | 36篇 |
2019年 | 30篇 |
2018年 | 32篇 |
2017年 | 31篇 |
2016年 | 37篇 |
2015年 | 16篇 |
2014年 | 28篇 |
2013年 | 56篇 |
2012年 | 9篇 |
2011年 | 15篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 19篇 |
2008年 | 14篇 |
2007年 | 26篇 |
2006年 | 19篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 26篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 9篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有635条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
142.
This paper examines two dimensions of concurrent verbal protocol validity. First, whether verbalization affects process and outcome (reactivity) was examined by comparing concurrent verbal protocol traces with those from a computer search process tracing method, the latter being a complete trace of information acquisition from experimental materials. Earlier findings that verbalization affects time were confirmed. However, verbalization did not affect amount and pattern of acquisition or accuracy of judgments. Second, whether concurrent verbal protocols are complete was examined by comparing concurrent verbal protocol and computer traces that were simultaneously obtained in a treatment in which subjects verbalized as they acquired information from the computer. The verbal traces less completely captured information acquisition behavior than computer search. This suggests that, although concurrent verbal protocols provide greater insight into decision behavior than computer search, the latter is a more reliable information-acquisition trace. Thus, if information acquisition is of primary interest and if computer search activities can be naturally integrated into performing the primary task, computer search is preferred to concurrent verbal protocols. However, if information use or retrieval from long-term memory is of primary interest, concurrent verbal protocols are preferred to computer search. Finally, this paper examined whether the simultaneous use of concurrent verbal protocols and computer search provides traces of information acquisition and use that are as complete as when each method is independently applied. Although computer search tended to limit subjects verbalizations of evaluative operators, this effect may be eliminated by practice on the computer prior to collecting data. 相似文献
143.
本文首先简要介绍了组织行为和领导力的研究特色和该领域国内外的研究现状。从中可以看出, 在过去的10多年中, 中国学者在该领域中进步快速, 并蕴含巨大的潜力。随后, 比较了国际和国内学术界的研究话题和采用的方法, 指出国内目前研究存在的问题。文章的第三部分介绍了当今组织行为和领导力领域在理论、方法和研究范式上的创新。针对当前中国经济和社会发展的现状, 文章指出了学者可以重点关注的领域。学者可以借鉴学术界已有的理论和方法, 选择恰当的角度对这些问题进行深入而系统的探索。为此, 本文第三部分还介绍了几项近年来具有重大影响的研究, 旨在启发学者关注中国社会的重要现象, 敏锐地发现适合本学科研究的问题, 并以严谨且有创造力的方式去开展研究, 进而对国际学术界做出贡献。 相似文献
144.
Rupert Hölzl Andreas Möltner Claus W. Neidig 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1999,34(4):269-284
Clinical and experimental evidence on referred pain and spinal-afferent convergence demonstrates a close relationship between
visceral and somatosensory perception, which is important for current models of symptom perception and central body representation.
The study uses a psychophysical approach to quantify these interactions at the perceptual level, taking into account problems
of comparable intermodal scaling and the role of awareness. An experiment on somatosensory masking of distension stimuli in
the colon is reported in which a multiple staircase method of forced choice discrimination with concurrent sensation ratings
was employed. Results showed perceptual masking of visceral by abdominal stimuli but not vice versa. The masking effect was
not enhanced by intratomal placement of the abdominal stimulus in the lower left quadrant. This contradicts the spinal sensory
convergence model and points to perceptual interactions at higher brain levels. Loglinear analysis of relations between discrimination
and subjective sensation revealed qualitative differences of somatovisceral perception at the preconscious as compared to
the conscious level. This argues for a two-process model of integrative body perception. 相似文献
145.
Ulf Kragh 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》1998,39(2):123-124
A paper by I. Zuber and B. Ekehammar in this Journal (38, 1997) is reviewed. The paper advocates that the non-veridical contents in the series of reports in the DMT should preferably be referred, not to psychoanalytic defence mechanisms, but to factors of stimulus order. The methods and the arguments of this investigation are critically examined. 相似文献
146.
147.
O. I. Larichev H. M. Moshkovich A. I. Mechitov David L. Olson 《Journal of Multi-Criteria Decision Analysis》1993,2(1):5-26
ZAPROS, a method to support rank ordering tasks using ordinal input from decision makers, is discussed and compared with a preference cone technique and the analytic hierarchy process (AHP). It provides a means to identify inconsistencies in ordinal decision tasks, yielding verification and explanation of results for partial ordering of a large set of alternatives. The results indicate that ZAPROS provides no less accuracy in task solution, while having some advantages from a behavioural point of view. Comparative analysis of the effectiveness of the methods under consideration in accordance with differences in task characteristics is carried out. 相似文献
148.
The taming (timing) of the states 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
149.
This paper presents a comparative study of three popular methods for multicriteria decision analysis based on a particular model of human preferential judgement. Since decisions are invariably made within a given context, we model relative preferences as ratios of increments or decrements in an interval on an axis of desirability. Next we sort the ratio magnitudes into a small number of categories, represented by numerical values on a geometric scale. We explain why the analytic hierarchy process (AHP) and the French collection of ELECTRE methods, typically based on pairwise comparison methods, are concerned with categories of ratio magnitudes, whereas the simple multiattribute rating technique (SMART) essentially uses orders of magnitude of these ratios. This phenomenon provides a common basis for the analysis of the methods in question and for a cross-validation of their results. We illustrate the approach via a well-known case study, the choice of a location for a nuclear power plant. We conclude by discussing the scope of the comparative study. © 1997 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
150.
John A. Glover 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1979,12(3):487-487
Reinforcement (team points) and practice were applied to the written work of 16 fifth graders on three operationally defined components of creative writing, fluency, flexibility, and originality.4 These three components were assessed via five response measures. Fluency was defined as the number of different ideas each student listed in an idea list prepared prior to writing an essay. Flexibility was measured in two ways, idea list flexibility and story flexibility. Idea list flexibility was defined as the number of different kinds of ideas which appeared in each day's list whereas story flexibility was defined as the number of different approaches to the topic that the child used in the story itself. Originality was also measured in two ways, idea list originality and story originality. Idea list originality was defined by the statistical infrequency of ideas in the children's lists of ideas. Story originality was determined by blind, subjective ratings of stories conducted by independent raters. All of the measures of creative responding were demonstrated to be under experimental control. The procedures also raised students' scores on Torrance's Tests of Thinking Creatively With Words on three variables, fluency, flexibility, and originality. Adoption for classroom use seems straightforward. 相似文献