首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   474篇
  免费   77篇
  国内免费   115篇
  2023年   7篇
  2022年   9篇
  2021年   16篇
  2020年   17篇
  2019年   26篇
  2018年   23篇
  2017年   25篇
  2016年   25篇
  2015年   11篇
  2014年   27篇
  2013年   51篇
  2012年   20篇
  2011年   32篇
  2010年   22篇
  2009年   32篇
  2008年   48篇
  2007年   56篇
  2006年   32篇
  2005年   30篇
  2004年   30篇
  2003年   30篇
  2002年   16篇
  2001年   15篇
  2000年   18篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   6篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   12篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   3篇
  1988年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1978年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
排序方式: 共有666条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
571.
The present study investigated whether people can combine two memory strategies to encode pairs of words more efficiently than with a single strategy, and age-related differences in such strategy combination. Young and older adults were asked to encode pairs of words (e.g., satellite-tunnel). For each item, participants were told to use either the interactive-imagery strategy (e.g., mentally visualising the two words and making them interact), the sentence-generation strategy (i.e., generate a sentence linking the two words), or with strategy combination (i.e., generating a sentence while mentally visualising it). Participants obtained better recall performance on items encoded with strategy combination than on items encoded with interactive-imagery or sentence-generation strategies. Moreover, we found age-related decline in such strategy combination. These findings have important implications to further our understanding of execution of memory strategies, and suggest that strategy combination occurs in a variety of cognitive domains.  相似文献   
572.
Whether older adults can compensate for their associative memory deficit by using memory strategies efficiently might depend on their general cognitive abilities. This study examined the moderating role of an IQ estimate on the beneficial effects of strategy instructions. A total of 142 participants (aged 18–85 years) received either intentional learning or strategy (“sentence generation”) instructions during encoding of word pairs. Whereas young adults with a lower IQ benefited from strategy instructions, those with a higher IQ did not, presumably because they already use strategies spontaneously. Older adults showed the opposite effect: following strategy instructions, older adults with a higher IQ showed a strong increase in memory performance (approximately achieving the level of younger adults), whereas older adults with a lower IQ did not, suggesting that they have difficulties implementing the provided strategies. These results highlight the importance of the role of IQ in compensating for the aging-related memory decline.  相似文献   
573.
彭义升  方平  姜媛 《心理科学》2011,34(6):1325-1331
随着脑成像等新技术的运用,关于情绪调节脑机制的研究有了很大进展。近期国外情绪调节脑机制的研究在理论模型上有了新发展;研究关注的情绪调节从有意情绪调节扩展到自动情绪调节;研究针对的情绪、情绪调节策略以及目标群体都有了进一步的扩展和深化;情绪调节脑机制的个体差异研究也有了新进展。本文试图对近期国外情绪调节脑机制研究的发展状况进行梳理,并展望未来研究方向,以期为国内研究提供参考。  相似文献   
574.
高丙成  刘儒德 《心理科学》2011,34(3):608-612
利用潜在剖面分析法和辨别分析法对374名初中生社会支持的类型进行考察,探讨不同社会支持类型初中生的特点,分析社会支持类型对学习动机、学习策略、成绩的影响。结果表明:(1)初中生社会支持分为亲密无间型、若即若离型和家庭疏远型3种。(2)亲密无间型初中生的社会支持最高,家庭疏远型初中生的社会支持尤其是家庭支持最低,而若即若离型初中生的社会支持处于亲密无间型和家庭疏远型之间。(3)社会支持类型对学习动机、学习策略、成绩有显著影响。  相似文献   
575.
林川  黄敏儿 《心理学报》2011,43(1):65-73
采用2×3组间设计, 检测特质应对(积极与消极)与展现规则(积极、消极、无规则)对情绪劳动的影响。结果表明, 积极应对引起更多深层动作; 积极规则下出现较少表层扮演(与消极应对比)。积极和消极规则都引起较多深层和表层动作。积极应对在积极规则下表层扮演较少(与消极规则比); 消极应对在积极规则下表层扮演较多(与消极规则比)。研究提示, 特质情绪性可能是调节展现规则与情绪劳动关系的重要原因。  相似文献   
576.
影响决策的情感因素——后悔理论的研究述评   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
情感因素在决策中起着重要的作用。本文对决策中最重要的情感因素——后悔因素的研究发展及其成果做了归纳总结,同时也将一些零散的发现整合在一起,使之更加系统化。后悔情绪对决策的影响作用不容忽视,作为决策理论之一的后悔理论在决策理论中的重要地位日益凸显出来。未来的研究将仍旧主要集中在其对决策的影响方面。后悔理论成为理性决策的一个可替代理论,已为时不远。  相似文献   
577.
The recognition heuristic (RH) claims that people base inferences on recognition only. This has been questioned by several studies which found that additional knowledge was influential. However, in some of these studies, participants' additional knowledge might have encompassed criterion knowledge thus rendering any inferential strategy superfluous. The present study was therefore designed to test the effect of criterion knowledge on use or non‐use of the RH. Eighty‐one participants made pair‐wise comparisons with respect to the size of Belgian cities and also provided estimates of the cities' actual size. We found that relative criterion knowledge (i.e., knowledge about the relative position of an object on the criterion dimension) did indeed play some role, but its exclusion left the main critical findings intact, nonetheless. We thus conclude that previous studies conducted in the paradigm of natural recognition should not be generally refuted by the argument of participants possessing criterion knowledge. Copyright © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
578.
下咽癌是一种预后较差的头颈部肿瘤。目前下咽癌治疗的临床决策还存在很多不规范和不合理的问题。运用循证医学的原则来指导和规范下咽癌治疗的临床决策,以期提高患者的治疗效果、降低医疗费用及提高生活质量。  相似文献   
579.
分娩是生殖健康的一个重要组成部分,如何减轻分娩疼痛,倡导生殖健康是现代医学领域研究的课题之一。本文通过反映我国无痛分娩率较低的现状,分析了影响无痛分娩推广的因素,指出了无痛分娩推广的重大临床意义及社会效益,并提出有效提高无痛分娩率的临床对策。  相似文献   
580.
Previous research has shown that positive affect (PA) is associated with the coping strategies of "searching for and finding positive meaning." The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between the coping strategy of "finding positive meaning" and PA using an intervention method. Additionally, inasmuch as previous research has revealed that PA is associated with physical and mental health status, the current study measured health status in order to carry out a preliminary test as to whether PA increased by the intervention can also improve health. Participants in the intervention group (Japanese graduate students; 13 men and 16 women) reported the most stressful event during the past 3 days and its positive meaning, while those in the control group (13 men and 15 women) reported the most stressful event alone. Both groups reported twice a week for 5 successive weeks using e-mails. Three questionnaires, the Japanese version of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule scales for measuring positive and negative affects (NA), the situational version of the General Coping Questionnaire for coping strategies, and the Japanese version of the General Health Questionnaire for health status, were administered to all participants just before the start of the intervention, after the conclusion of the intervention, and at the 5-week follow-up session. Results revealed that the "finding positive meaning" coping strategy and PA were enhanced by the intervention in the follow-up for men and women with no changes in NA. For the health scales of the General Health Questionnaire, no significant group-related effects were observed. Thus, the intervention had no significant influence on NA or health status. Implications of these findings for future research are discussed along with a few limitations in this study.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号