首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   175篇
  免费   33篇
  国内免费   32篇
  2023年   4篇
  2022年   1篇
  2021年   1篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   7篇
  2017年   5篇
  2016年   9篇
  2015年   7篇
  2014年   4篇
  2013年   21篇
  2012年   6篇
  2011年   10篇
  2010年   4篇
  2009年   9篇
  2008年   19篇
  2007年   15篇
  2006年   15篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   12篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   4篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   5篇
  1999年   7篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   1篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   1篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   3篇
  1984年   1篇
  1983年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1979年   2篇
排序方式: 共有240条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
People with grapheme-color synesthesia perceive specific colors when viewing different letters or numbers. Previous studies have suggested that synesthetic color experience can be bistable when induced by an ambiguous character. However, the exact relationship between processes underlying the identity of an alphanumeric character and the experience of the induced synesthetic color has not been examined. In the present study, we explored this by focusing on the temporal relation of inducer identification and color emergence using inducers whose identity could be rendered ambiguous upon rotation of the characters. Specifically, achromatic alphabetic letters (W/M) and digits (6/9) were presented at varying angles to 9 grapheme-color synesthetes. Results showed that grapheme identification and synesthetically perceived grapheme color covary with the orientation of the test stimulus and that synesthetes were slower naming the experienced color than identifying the character, particularly at intermediate angles where ambiguity was greatest.  相似文献   
142.
Abstract

This article presents research into the mother–child relationship according to psychoanalytic theory. It is based on data from groups of mothers who have children with special needs in the context of their experiences with inclusive education policies.  相似文献   
143.
Wegener  Claudia B. 《Studia Logica》2002,70(3):339-352
A Priestley duality is developed for the variety j of all modal lattices. This is achieved by restricting to j a known Priestley duality for the variety of all bounded distributive lattices with a meet-homomorphism. The variety j was first studied by R. Beazer in 1986.The dual spaces of free modal lattices are constructed, paralleling P.R. Halmos' construction of the dual spaces of free monadic Boolean algebras and its generalization, by R. Cignoli, to distributive lattices with a quantifier.  相似文献   
144.
We study the monoid of primitive recursive functions and investigate a onestep construction of a kind of exact completion, which resembles that of the familiar category of modest sets, except that the partial equivalence relations which serve as objects are recursively enumerable. As usual, these constructions involve the splitting of symmetric idempotents.  相似文献   
145.
对中国北京市和日本京都市的各一所幼儿园小班幼儿园团体进行了自然观察,利用动物行为这的社会等级距等方法分析幼儿园之间围绕一定资源所发生的相互作用,考察了幼儿团体中的所有关系是否由它们之间的支配-从属关系或等级制度所规定。所得主要结果如下:(1)中日幼儿团体的社会等级矩阵的稳固性与直线性都比较低;(2)他们在社会等级矩阵上的等级和由班主任评定的争夺之量分数,领导分数之间都没有显著相关,(3)从幼儿之间  相似文献   
146.
M. Azar 《Argumentation》1999,13(1):97-114
Rhetorical Structure Theory (RST), as a tool for analyzing written texts, is particularly appropriate for analyzing argumentative texts. The distinction that RST makes between the part of a text that realizes the primary goal of the writer, termed nucleus, and the part that provides supplementary material, termed satellite, is crucial for the analysis of argumentative texts.The paper commences by determining the concept of argument relation (argument + conclusion) and by briefly presenting RST. It continues by identifying five of RST's rhetorical relations of the satellite/nucleus schema (Evidence, Motivation, Justify, Antithesis, Concession) as five argument relations, each being, logically or pragmatically, a special kind of argument: Evidence being a supportive argument, Motivation an incentive argument, Justify a justifier argument, and Antithesis and Concession persuader arguments. To illustrate, an analysis of three short texts concludes the paper.  相似文献   
147.
从全国29个省、市、自治区回收的1433份有效问卷的统计结果显示,人们的人际关系与身体健康和心理健康是密切关联、相互影响的,人际关系高度影响身体健康和心理健康,但其对于心理健康的影响作用,比之对身体健康更大。  相似文献   
148.
The principles of ontological parity and ordinality have distinct functions in Buchler's ontology. Ontological parity could be independently subscribed to, whereas ordinality signals the positive conception of the nature of reality as irreducibly complex or indefinitely related, which Buchler's metaphysical system seeks to articulate. Both principles inform Buchler's system, but each has a distinctive function. They are not, I suggest, necessarily at odds with one another, as some critics claim. I do identify several difficulties that follow from (1) the level of generality claimed by Buchler and (2) the claim of irreducible complexity or indefinite relatedness.  相似文献   
149.
学习不良少年对父母评价的认知   总被引:11,自引:0,他引:11  
雷雳 《心理学报》1998,31(1):64-69
运用问卷测量,对普通学校的272名11 ̄15岁的少年学生及其家长分组进行学习不良与非学习不良的比较,考察了学生对父母评价的认知,以及父母评价的特点及其关系。结果表明:①学习不良学生对父母评价的认知与非学习不良学生存在明显差异;②学习不良学生对父母评价的认知没有性别差异。③除亲子关系维度有显著的二次趋势外,其它方面均无显著的发展趋势,但学习不良学生在13岁前后与非学习不良学生的差距更为突出。④学习不  相似文献   
150.
Matching, undermatching, and overmatching in studies of choice   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1       下载免费PDF全文
Almost all of 103 sets of data from 23 different studies of choice conformed closely to the equation: log (B(1)/B(2)) = a log (r(1)/r(2)) + log b, where B(1) and B(2) are either numbers of responses or times spent at Alternatives 1 and 2, r(1) and r(2) are the rates of reinforcement obtained from Alternatives 1 and 2, and a and b are empirical constants. Although the matching relation requires the slope a to equal 1.0, the best-fitting values of a frequently deviated from this. For B(1) and B(2) measured as numbers of responses, a tended to fall short of 1.0 (undermatching). For B(1) and B(2) measured as times, a fell to both sides of 1.0, with the largest mode at about 1.0. Those experiments that produced values of a for both responses and time revealed only a rough correspondence between the two values; a was often noticeably larger for time. Statistical techniques for assessing significance of a deviation of a from 1.0 suggested that values of a between .90 and 1.11 can be considered good approximations to matching. Of the two experimenters who contributed the most data, one generally found undermatching, while the other generally found matching. The difference in results probably arises from differences in procedure. The procedural variations that lead to undermatching appear to be those that produce (a) asymmetrical pausing that favors the poorer alternative; (b) systematic temporal variation in preference that favors the poorer alternative; and (c) patterns of responding that involve changing over between alternatives or brief bouts at the alternatives.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号