全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1990篇 |
免费 | 429篇 |
国内免费 | 26篇 |
专业分类
2445篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 26篇 |
2023年 | 43篇 |
2022年 | 13篇 |
2021年 | 65篇 |
2020年 | 78篇 |
2019年 | 83篇 |
2018年 | 89篇 |
2017年 | 94篇 |
2016年 | 77篇 |
2015年 | 63篇 |
2014年 | 70篇 |
2013年 | 214篇 |
2012年 | 62篇 |
2011年 | 81篇 |
2010年 | 60篇 |
2009年 | 62篇 |
2008年 | 70篇 |
2007年 | 57篇 |
2006年 | 68篇 |
2005年 | 53篇 |
2004年 | 66篇 |
2003年 | 58篇 |
2002年 | 59篇 |
2001年 | 48篇 |
2000年 | 39篇 |
1999年 | 45篇 |
1998年 | 41篇 |
1997年 | 32篇 |
1996年 | 36篇 |
1995年 | 27篇 |
1994年 | 32篇 |
1993年 | 34篇 |
1992年 | 33篇 |
1991年 | 34篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 23篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 27篇 |
1985年 | 23篇 |
1984年 | 24篇 |
1983年 | 20篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 30篇 |
1980年 | 30篇 |
1979年 | 26篇 |
1978年 | 43篇 |
1977年 | 43篇 |
1976年 | 41篇 |
1975年 | 18篇 |
排序方式: 共有2445条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
112.
A group of young children (mean age: 2.5 yr) were instructed to follow different requests by a teacher in a day-care setting. Experiment I verified that mean group instruction following was low (10%) despite the opportunity for “observational learning”, i.e., the group of 12 children could watch a nonreinforced adult comply with the teacher's request. In Experiment II, when positive consequences were provided contingent on the adult model's behavior, mean group instruction following was relatively unaffected (14%). When direct reinforcement was given to four peer models, each for several sessions, the individual performances of three of the four peer models was elevated (from 50% to 80%); however, the mean performance of the remaining nonreinforced children (N = 7) was only moderately affected (21%). When reinforcement contingencies were again changed, so that each group member was provided direct, but intermittent reinforcement, mean group performance increased substantially to levels of over 70%. Once instruction following was high, presentation of reinforcement only to one peer model sufficed to maintain performance whereas earlier, this same vicarious reinforcement procedure had failed to establish group compliance. The maintenance of instruction-following behavior when reinforcement was applied solely to one child was interpreted mainly in terms of a high resistance to extinction following a history of intermittent reinforcement rather than a “vicarious”- or “self”-reinforcement mechanism. Finally, removal and re-introduction of group intermittent reinforcement, respectively, lowered performance (to levels of 40%) and elevated (to levels of 65%) the group's performance. 相似文献
113.
本研究通过设置对空间注意资源的分配实施强行操作的情形下,探讨了主、次作业客体形状一致性和次作业客体间距对乏空间注意区次作业客体颜色、形状特征的检测及对特征错觉性结合的影响。实验结果表明:(1)分散空间注意使次作业区域注意减弱,对客体及其颜色和形状特征的识别均产生明显影响.而且对形状的影响又大于颜色;(2)特征错觉性结合率,主、次作业客体形状一致时比客体形状不一致时明显增大;(3)次作业客体间距对特征错觉性结合率无明显影响,但客体形状一致性和客体间距之间存在明显交互作用. 相似文献
114.
该研究试图考察北京大学生的整体自尊、多元自尊的特点,探讨整体自尊的结构,考察它与抑郁等消极情绪的关系。研究采用华人多元自尊量表、华人整体自尊量表,在北京地区18所高校中,随机选取300名大学生施测。结果显示:1)多元自尊存在性别差异和年级差异;2)在北京大学生的多元自尊中,个人独立与能力自尊、人际关系与人缘自尊以及家世背景与经济自尊是自尊结构中的主要维度,能够有效预测整体自尊;3)整体自尊与焦虑、抑郁、以及消极情绪的强度与频率都存在显著的负相关。 相似文献
115.
This study examined the effect of stimulus movement on localization probability and latency during attention and inattention. Forty infants, 10 each at 8, 14, 20, and 26 weeks of age were presented with a central stimulus. Then, a peripheral stimulus was presented (static or dynamic checkerboard). Stimulus movement did not affect localization probability. Infants localized the dynamic peripheral stimulus more quickly than the static peripheral stimulus when there was no focal stimulus. Focal stimulus attention attenuated this difference in localization latency between static and dynamic stimuli. Signal detection analysis showed that sensitivity to the peripheral stimulus increased over this age range along with a decrease in the bias against responding. The effects of attention were on response bias rather than stimulus sensitivity. These results imply attention affected the localization response to the peripheral stimulus but did not affect the sensitivity of the sensory and perceptual pathways to peripheral stimuli. 相似文献
116.
Conditions under which children experience inhibitory difficulty with a "button-press" go/no-go task
Go/no-go tasks seem to provide a simple marker of inhibitory development in young children. Children are told to respond to one stimulus on go trials but to make no response to another stimulus on no-go trials; responding on no-go trials is assumed to reflect a failure to inhibit the go response. However, there is evidence to suggest that a type of go/no-go task, which we call the "button-press" task, does not require inhibition. We investigated the conditions under which young children (M=3 years 6 months, N=120) experience inhibitory difficulty with this type of task. The data suggest that the speed of stimulus presentation is crucial and that other studies using this type of task have presented the stimuli too briefly. The importance of establishing the inhibitory credentials of a task before it is used as a marker of inhibitory control is emphasized. 相似文献
117.
The personality of meaning in life: Associations between dimensions of life meaning and the Big Five
《The journal of positive psychology》2013,8(1):34-43
The goal of the current study was to identify aspects of personality that are associated with different ways in which people find meaning in life. This was achieved using constrained principal component analysis (CPCA) on data from 322 university students, who completed the Sources of Meaning and Meaning in Life questionnaire and the Big Five Aspects Scale. CPCA demonstrated that personality traits and life meaning are associated, but not redundant, with one another. Specifically, respondents with high scores on lower-level aspects of Openness to Experience tended to derive meaning from questioning, learning and challenging tradition, whereas those with high scores on aspects of Conscientiousness and Extraversion tended to derive meaning from success at work, health, and family. Results suggest that personality traits are associated with variations in the domains used to derive meaning in life, and demonstrate the utility of CPCA as an innovative statistical technique for the study of individual differences. 相似文献
118.
《The journal of positive psychology》2013,8(2):105-127
People are often profoundly moved by the virtue or skill of others, yet psychology has little to say about the ‘other-praising’ family of emotions. Here we demonstrate that emotions such as elevation, gratitude, and admiration differ from more commonly studied forms of positive affect (joy and amusement) in many ways, and from each other in a few ways. The results of studies using recall, video induction, event-contingent diary, and letter-writing methods to induce other-praising emotions suggest that: elevation (a response to moral excellence) motivates prosocial and affiliative behavior, gratitude motivates improved relationships with benefactors, and admiration motivates self-improvement. Mediation analyses highlight the role of conscious emotion between appraisals and motivations. Discussion focuses on implications for emotion research, interpersonal relationships, and morality. 相似文献
119.
Starla L. Dallesasse 《Ethics & behavior》2013,23(6):419-428
Graduate students typically serve in a wide variety of positions throughout their course of study in an academic setting. Because of the large number of roles assumed, graduate students may encounter several ethical dilemmas unique to the academic setting, particularly dilemmas regarding nonsexual multiple relationships. Although the topic of multiple relationships and psychologists has been extensively discussed in the literature, fewer articles have focused on the potential for nonsexual multiple relationships between graduate assistants, practicum students, and undergraduate students. This article focuses on the unique ethical dilemmas and challenges experienced by graduate assistants completing their practica at University Counseling Centers and provides recommendations for evaluating and managing nonsexual multiple relationships with undergraduate students. 相似文献
120.
组织器官微循环灌注障碍是MODS发生的重要机制,胃肠道是机体最早发生缺血的器官。胃PHi能敏感反映MODS发生过程中胃肠黏膜缺氧情况,对MODS具有预警作用。目前测定PHi方法分盐水张力计法和连续气态张力计法,效果较可靠。本文通过大量查阅文献,结合临床经验,较系统地阐述了pHi在MODS监测中的意义、原理、方法、安全性及应用前景,为及早采取措施,防治MODS提供理论依据和临床参考。 相似文献